Thursday, May 6, 2021

Emotion Reactivity and Regulation in Targeting Depression

From a 2017 WHO report, it was concluded that depression impacted 322 million people worldwide. For every one that underwent treatment for depression, 45-65% never reached remission. Even those who did reach remission had about a 50% chance of relapsing after 1-2 years. Mainstream interventions for treating depression have focused on repairing associated negative emotions, but do little in increasing positive emotions. Thus, interventions that target impairments in positive emotions could be critical in diminishing depression. Depression is related to impaired emotion reactivity and regulation, with emotion reactivity being an initial baseline response and emotion regulation being the process that influences an initial emotional response. However, there’s been difficulty disentangling emotion reactivity and regulation, and doing so is important in understanding how current treatments for depression should be refined. In the talk “Individual differences and neural correlates of emotion reactivity and regulation: potential intervention targets in depression” presented by Kahrilas (2021), the researcher presented three studies conducted to disentangle emotion reactivity and regulation to bring us closer to conceiving neuroscience-informed treatments for depression.

The first study was in regards to the concept of savoring the moment and linking affectivity and depression. Savoring capacity is the capacity for an individual to attend to, appreciate, and enhance the positive experience of one’s life. As such, one’s savoring capacity is the index of one’s ability to regulate their positive emotions. When you are savoring, you may anticipate future events before they occur (anticipating), or you can attend to and appreciate positive events as they are occurring in the present moment (savoring the moment), or you can reminisce upon positive events after they have occurred (reminiscing). Regardless of the temporal focus, each of these temporal domains of savoring is indicative of one’s ability to savor emotions in the present moment. In this study, 1,618 participants were measured with the Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI), the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ), the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9), and the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ). Results showed a positive relationship between positive affectivity and each of the three temporal domains of savoring, and a negative relationship between positive affectivity and depression. A negative relationship between negative affectivity and each of the three temporal domains of savoring was also found, and a positive relationship between negative affectivity and depression was observed. Moreover, savoring the moment was the sole predictor of depression, suggesting that momentary savoring has a higher specificity to depression and might be a more effective intervention target.  

In the second study, the neural chronometry of positive and negative emotion reactivity and regulation was investigated. The main goal of this study was to disentangle the constructs of emotion reactivity and emotion regulation. Since the time course of reactivity and regulation likely overlap, electroencephalography (EEG) is an efficient psychophysiological measure of electrical cortical activity by event-related potentials (ERPs) with millisecond (ms) temporal resolution that can be used to evaluate the chronometry of reactivity and regulation processes. The present study utilized EEG methods to determine the neural time course of emotion reactivity and regulation to provide knowledge as to how these processes are implicated and how they can be altered in depression. A principal component approach (PCA) was used to measure ERPs, which is a dimension reduction technique that takes a large number of variables and reduces them to a smaller set of variables that constitute linear combinations of the original data set. The most common waveform from PCA is the Late Positive Potential (LPP), which is a positive slow wave EEG component observed as early as 300 ms following stimulus onset. LPP is an established index of evaluative congruency, valence, and arousal in response to visual stimuli, and is selectively enhanced in response to positive and negative visual stimuli relative to neutral stimuli. The study also examined early visual processes, seen with the N170 (a negative peak occurring at 170 ms) and EPN (negative peak occurring around 200-300 ms), since these components have been studied in previous literature on facial processing, showing that these components are enhanced in response to images of negative and positive faces. The present study used 120 standardized images from the open-affective standardized image set, which are images normed on valence (unpleasant and pleasant) and arousal (not aroused and aroused). Using the image sets, there were three distinct categories: 40 positive, 40 negative, 40 neutral. For the positive and negative image sets, there were three sets of instructions for participants: increase or decrease the emotional intensity they felt in response to the images, or passively view the images. For neutral images, participants were told to just passively view them. After viewing the images, participants rated how positive or negative they felt in the present moment on a 1-7 Likert Scale. They also reported the arousal of the emotion and the difficulty of the task. The chronometry of reactivity unfolded from 162 ms to 740 ms, with stable arousal and valence effect throughout the time course. In terms of regulation, negative regulatory processes unfolded earlier at 124 ms to 259 ms, whereas positive regulatory processes occurred later from 259 ms to 740 ms. These findings are important because different types of psychopathology might manifest as dysregulating positive or negative emotion regulation or reactivity, suggesting that we might need different intervention processes depending on the symptomatology. 

The third study presented summarized early neural activity as an indicator of the brightening effect. There are three theories of emotion reactivity in depression: positive attenuation (reduced reactivity in response to positive stimuli), negative potentiation (enhanced reactivity in response to negative stimuli), and emotion context insensitivity (ECI; reduced reactivity in response to positive and negative stimuli). Within the realm of lab-based research, ECI has emerged to be the most common finding; however, this is not the case in a different type of research called ecological momentary assessment. With this assessment, researchers have participants download an app on their phones and have them complete questionnaires throughout the day. A different pattern of emotion reactivity and depression emerges with this research, such that there is enhanced reactivity in response to positive stimuli in those with depression as opposed to those without depression. Much research of depression and emotion reactivity in psychopathology looks at depression as a heterogeneous cluster of symptoms, and findings from study 1 here show that positive affectivity shows specificity to depression, so Kahrilas was interested in how neural activity changes as a function of positive affectivity specifically, rather than depression as a whole. Previous research showed that those with depression tend to exhibit smaller amplitudes in response to both positive and negative images, which is consistent with the ECI view, where we see less reactivity in response to visual stimuli regardless of if they are positive or negative. Previous studies also show that those endorsing lower levels of positive affectivity tend to exhibit attenuated reactivity in response to negative and positive images. Moreover, previous literature employing structural equation modeling found a model corresponding to ECI that found associations between viewing pleasant and unpleasant images and depressive symptoms that were approaching significance. Following these findings, the present study hypothesized that reactivity in response to positive and negative stimuli would be positively associated with positive affectivity, such that those with depression (those with lower positive affectivity) would report smaller amplitudes in response to those images, and that an ECI model would best map onto the findings. Study 3 used the same EEG paradigm as Study 2. Study 3 also used the same sample as study 2, but included a new sample of participants from a different study in conjunction with the current impact lab that was recruited based on depressive symptoms. More specifically, participants recruited were those that endorsed moderate levels of depression, and Kahrilas harmonized them with the previous sample that was not recruited on this basis of depression. This introduces more variance of measures of central tendency, which results in a greater dimensional perspective of the concept of interest. A positive component peak at 371 ms at bilateral occipital electrode sites was found. Negative and positive images elicited augmented amplitude relative to neutral images. Another positive component occurring later in the time course was found, peaking at about 736 ms at a cluster of bilateral centroparietal electrode sites. A negative component peaking at 257 ms was also observed. For PCA in study 3, there was no peak at 162 ms that was observed in study 2, suggesting that there might be alterations in the early visual components with the introduction of depressive symptoms. For the 257 ms component results, the association between the residual variance of the positive viewing condition and positive affectivity was strongly correlated. However, there was no association found between negative viewing conditions and positive affectivity. Since the EPN is a negative-going component, this means that the positive association is in the opposite direction than the ECI model hypothesized. This is explained by the brightening effect model, which was found in the ecological momentary assessment literature, that says there's augmented emotional reactivity in response to positive images as a function of depression. The brightening effect model was the best model for the 257 ms component, which draws the association between positive images and positive affectivity, meaning that early visual ERP components might show specificity to those with low positive affectivity. For the 371 ms and 736 ms component results, nothing outperformed the measurement model here, meaning that these later ERP components may be independent of internalizing symptoms. Conclusions from study 3 were that only the positive viewing condition for the early visual 257 ms component was negatively related to positive affectivity, consistent with the brightening effect. Furthermore, later ERP components were not related to internalizing symptoms. Therefore, interventions that target early neural processes may strengthen positive affectivity and alleviate depression. 

Overall, Kahrilas’ Study 1 found that momentary savoring might diminish depressive symptoms for those with low positive affectivity and high negative affectivity. Study 2 disentangled positive emotion reactivity and regulation. Neural reactivity unfolded from 162 ms to 740 ms with stable arousal and valence effects. Negative regulatory processes unfolded at 124 ms to 259 ms, and positive regulatory processes occurred later from 259 ms to 740 ms. Study 3 found that early neural activity is related to positive affectivity. These studies collectively bring us closer to conceiving neurobiological treatments for depression. 

Similar to Kahrilas’ Study 2 that investigated the neural chronometry of positive and negative emotion reactivity and regulation to disentangle the constructs of emotion reactivity and emotion regulation, Ebneabbasi et al.’s (2021) “Emotion processing and regulation in major depressive disorder: A 7T resting-state fMRI study” went off the concept that debilitated emotion processing (EP) and emotion regulation (ER) are key factors in the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder (MDD), with biased processing and impaired regulation of affective stimuli. EP and emotion reactivity are equal. Disturbances of EP are seen with excessive attention toward negative events, and disturbances of ER correspond to insufficient suppression of negative affect and incompetent savoring of positive ones. Like Kahrilas’ previous issue with disentangling EP and ER, the present study utilized regional amplitude of low frequency fluctuations (ALFF) and whole-brain functional connectivity (FC) of EP- and ER-related areas compared between 32 healthy controls (HC) and 20 MDD patients to discern if EP- and ER-related areas are linked to regulatory behavior and whether this relation is impaired in MDD. Previous literature found that higher amygdala reactivity led to greater prefrontal activity, thus resulting in greater regulatory behavior. In MDD, previous analyses found hyperactivity of the amygdala and hypoactivity of the lateral prefrontal cortex with exposure to negative stimuli, suggesting an augmented emotional reactivity and decreased downregulation of debilitated amygdala reactivity, offsetting prefrontal recruitment, and meta-analytic disparities in MDD. Moreover, it was examined whether EP-related areas are predictors of ER-related areas and regulatory behavior in both experimental groups, and the brain-behavior associations between EP- and ER-related brain areas and depression severity were assessed. Results showed that affective areas were regionally and/or connectively impaired in MDD patients, and EP- and ER-related areas are disturbed in MDD patients. Overloading emotional reactivity in the amygdala has the potential to inversely affect cognitive control processes in prefrontal cortices, resulting in decreased regulatory actions. The amygdala plays a role in encoding relevant stimuli, provoking affective emotional responses. Higher amygdala activation was also found with exposure to negative stimuli in MDD patients, with prolonged processing of negative information, confirming Kahrilas’ finding that negative affectivity is positively associated with depression. Following the Kahrilas study that momentary savoring may diminish depressive symptoms, the current study found a decreased FC between the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and intraparietal sulcus in MDD patients, suggesting an inability of MDD patients to rely on savoring capacity and attend to positive emotions in the present. Altogether, these findings provide new insights on the underlying neural correlates of affective dysfunctions experienced with depression, which was a future goal of the Kahrilas study. 



                References

Ebneabbasi, A., Mahdipour, M., Nejati, V., Li, M., Liebe, T., Colic, L., Leutritz, A. L., Vogel, M., Zarei, M., Walter, M., & Tahmasian, M. (2021). Emotion processing and regulation in major depressive disorder: A 7T resting-state fMRI study. Human brain mapping, 42(3), 797-810.


Foti, D., Hajcak, G., & Dien, J. (2009). Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology, 46(3), 521-530.


Hill, K. E., South, S. C., Egan, R. P., & Foti, D. (2019). Abnormal emotional reactivity in depression: Contrasting theoretical models using neurophysiological data. Biological psychology, 141, 35-43.


Silton, R. L., Kahrilas, I. K., Skymba, H. V., Smith, J., Bryant, F. B., & Heller, W. (2020). Regulating positive emotions: Implications for promoting well-being in individuals with depression. Emotion, 20(1), 93-97.


Wednesday, May 5, 2021

Potential Caveat For Early ASD Diagnosis In Infants

     Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can produce a variety of different social and behavioral challenges upon those who are affected. Depending on the spectrum of Autism, people with ASD learn, think, and problem-solve differently than those who are not on the spectrum. ASD typically involves early brain overgrowth affecting several cortical and subcortical regions such as prefrontal and temporal cortices. These affected areas mediate the symptoms that a person with ASD experiences. Despite the increasing number of diagnoses, ASD is hard to diagnose because it is not detectable by medical tests but instead dependent on developmental and behavioral tracking. ASD can sometimes be detected at 18-months but many individuals do not receive a confirmed diagnosis until they are older. Some early signs of ASD include: avoiding eye contact, having little interest in other children or caretakers, limited display of language, or getting upset by minor changes in routine (“Screening and Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder”). 

In the article, "Face-sensitive brain responses in the first year of life", Guy and colleagues sought to examine the changes in neural response of infants to facial stimuli. Previous studies on adults have shown that the N170, located in the middle and posterior fusiform gyrus, is linked to the processing of facial stimuli. The study analyzed ERPs of infants that were face sensitive which involves P1, N290, P400, and Nc. Over a hundred infants were recruited and presented with stimuli consisting of pictures of faces and objects on various backgrounds. During the three experimental procedures, the ECG electrodes of the infants were used to determine the attention of the infants while the EEG electrodes to analyze the neural responses and calculate the amplitudes. The results depicted that the amplitudes of the ERPs increased in older infants 9+ months. The results of this study have lead Guy and colleagues to explore the possibility of using ERP analysis in diagnosing ASD in infants. 

In a separate study by Stoner and colleagues, "Patches of Disorganization in the Neocortex of Children with Autism", examined the neocortical architecture of children after the onset of autism by utilizing RNA in situ hybridization with a panel of layer- and cell-type–specific molecular markers to phenotype ASD. The study observed that children with ASD displayed focal disruptions of cortical laminar architecture in their cortices. Their data supported the idea of a potential dysregulation of layer formation and layer-specific neuronal differentiation at prenatal developmental stages of infants with ASD. In Guy’s study, the ERPs of older infants displaying proper development in facial stimuli response were larger in comparison to younger infants or infants that may have ASD. Stoner’s study could serve as supplemental information to support the results of Guy’s study as to why the ERPs were larger in normal developing infants rather than ASD infants. This could also be a potential caveat in future studies for early ASD diagnosis in order to create reliable medical tests. 


Citations

  • Conte, Stefania, et al. “Face-Sensitive Brain Responses in the First Year of Life.” NeuroImage, vol. 211, 2020, p. 116602., doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116602. 

  • Stoner, Rich, et al. “Patches of Disorganization in the Neocortex of Children with Autism.” New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 370, no. 13, 2014, pp. 1209–1219., doi:10.1056/nejmoa1307491. 

  • “Signs and Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 29 Mar. 2021, www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/signs.html. 

Neural Disorders and Potential HFS Treatment

  The medical field is always evolving, and researchers are constantly in search of new and improved treatment methods. For many decades, even to this day, physicians treat neural disorders such as Parkinson’s disease and epilepsy mostly using medication. However, a more recent focus on magnetic stimulation and high frequency stimulation (HFS), is showing promising results and proving to be very effective at successfully treating neural disorders. Thanks to researchers such as Ye et al., as well as Skach et al, who have worked diligently to examine new treatment methods such as microscopic magnetic stimulation as well as high frequency stimulation (HFS) respectively, physicians can hopefully start utilizing these faster and more effective treatment methods in the very near future.

In the research article “Axonal blockage with microscopic magnetic stimulation” Ye et al. worked to examine the feasibility of axonal blockage by the miniature coil. In order to test the effectiveness of this treatment method, the researchers used a combination of electrophysiological experiments and computational modeling. In order to accomplish this, they specifically designed a system that can deliver sufficient electric current of various frequency and intensity into a commercially available miniature coil. The researchers recorded axonal conductance in the buccal nerve II of the buccal ganglion in Aplysia californica while applying magnetic stimulation to the axon. As a result of this experiment, it was found that high frequency stimulation (HFS) with the miniature coil suppressed action potentials generated by antidromic stimulation. Antidromic stimulation of the BN2 activates B3, B6, B9, and B10 neurons in the buccal ganglion. Furthermore, HFS with miniature coil suppressed action potentials generated by specific soma activation. These results suggest that a population of axons, such as the 2nd largest units in BN2, have been inhibited by the miniature coil. The results of this study are very interesting and exciting. With further research this could lead to a groundbreaking treatment alternative, potentially more effective than standard medication.

Another study “Simulation Study of Intermittent Axonal Block and Desynchronization Effect Induced by High-Frequency Stimulation of Electrical Pulses” Skach et al shared a similar interest in using HFS in order to achieve axonal blockages. As a result, the researchers aimed to study the axonal responses during HFS. In order to do that, they developed a computational model of myelinated axons to simulate sequences of action potentials generated in single and multiple axons by stimulations, and applying the stimulations using a point source of current pulses with a frequency of 50–200 Hz, while taking into account the accumulation of potassium ions in the peri-axonal spaces. As a result of this experiment, it was found that there was an increase of potassium ions in the extracellular space, which generates intermittent depolarization blocks in the axons during the HFS treatment. This results in the axons firing at a much lower rate, and causes asynchronous firing of action potential on axon bundles. This could lead to the suppression of pathological synchronization of target nuclei by generating asynchronous activity in the neurons downstream. These results provide a very promising look into the therapeutic effects of DBS, which will hopefully result in the development of various effective treatments for neural disorders.

Looking at the results of these new treatment methods for neural disorders is very exciting. Hopefully future studies will further examine the effectiveness of such treatments and lead to a standardized HFS treatment for neural disorders. Studies such as these are vital for the field of neuroscience as they look into the effectiveness of our current techniques and ask what can be done to improve them. This type of outlook helps push the field forward, which in term helps medical professionals do a better job at diagnosing and treating patients with a variety of illnesses. With further research, these two methods have great potential to deepen our understanding of neural disorders and how they can be treated effectively using methods such as HFS and magnetic stimulation.

Sleep and Memory an Ongoing Investigation

 Sleep is an essential function in our lives. Sleep promotes cognitive performance, both mental and physical health, development, immune system functions, etc. Sleep also has shown to have vast benefits for memory consolidation, as well as promotes learning. During sleep, the body is able to repair itself and prepare itself for the following day. The effects sleep has on our learning/memory capabilities are still a subject of debate as determining the extensive working anatomy of memory and how sleep correlates to them.

While we associate things like cellular repair, physical development, etc with sleep, we still are left to understand how sleep promotes memory consolidation, as well as what mechanisms are at play during sleep. One such hypothesis is that sleep supports memory consolidation, perhaps when hippocampal neurons replay patterns of firing that were experienced during learning. In the article, How the brain consolidates memory during deep sleep”, researchers at the University of California were able to use a computational model looking at electrical activity in the brain during slow-wave sleep (deep sleep). The model displayed that patterns of slow oscillations in the cortex are influenced by hippocampal sharp-wave ripples, which determined synaptic changes in the cortex. Going off the theory of hippocampal neurons replaying patterns can be applied here as the model showed these synaptic changes affected patterns of slow oscillations, which replays a specific firing sequence of cortical neurons. Yina Wei explained how the inputs from the hippocampus determined the spatial and temporal pattern of the slow oscillations. She also explained that “by influencing the nature of these oscillations, this hippocampal input activates selective memories during deep sleep and causes a replay of specific memories.” These findings can be connected to other experiments and research into memory consolidation as well as further understanding mechanisms of sleep and memory.


This research conducted by Dr. Yina Wei and the team at UC Riverside can be used alongside the research of Laura Shanahan and Jay A. Gottfried. In their review article, “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain”, they referenced research regarding targeted memory reactivation (TMR), where research by Rasch et al. 2007 found that odor stimulation could be used to enhance consolidation of declarative memories. They had subjects learn the location of several card pairs. During this, they were exposed to a rose odor (phenylethyl alcohol), and then they would go to sleep. During sleep, they would be re-exposed to the odor, and then upon waking they were asked to recall the locations of the items. They found that during the slow-wave sleep showed to have the greatest response to the odor and enhancement of recall. As displayed by the research of the UC Riverside team they also saw that there was activation during deep sleep (slow-wave sleep). They also employed fMRI on hippocampal activation. The results showed that the rose odor activated the hippocampus to a greater extent during deep sleep than it did during wakefulness, which mirrors that computational model used by the UC Riverside group.


Both these studies identify non-REM sleep (slow-wave sleep) to be the most active and likely area of memory activation, but a recent study done at the University of Tsukuba provides insight into the possible role REM sleep plays on memory consolidation. The research article, “Memory consolidation during REM sleep”, found that adult-born neurons in the hippocampus may be responsible for memory consolidation during REM sleep. The researchers exposed mice to a context-specific fear memory task. They then recorded activity in the adult-born neurons across the stages of memory. They found that these neurons were most active during REM sleep after the memory task. They also found consolidation of contextual fear memories was impaired upon optogenetic silencing of young ABNs. This can be used alongside the research talked about in the Shanahan article about how odors can promote fear extinction during sleep. This is important as the other research points back to memory consolidation taking place in the slow-wave sleep while this research points to memory consolidation in REM sleep.


The three research articles all cover a different aspect of sleep and memory and have building blocks upon each other to further research into how sleep affects our memory system. The computational model supports many findings of increased activation during slow-wave sleep which is believed to be where memory-enhancing effects take place. While the odor experiment did not stimulate memory-enhancing effects during REM sleep, it is not fully conclusive that memory consolidation or activation can not take place during REM sleep. The REM sleep article offers insight into the role of adult-born neurons aiding in memory consolidation during REM, which could be further studied and applied to previous experiments. The hypothesis of hippocampal neurons replaying patterns of firing from learning also gains much support from the articles and experiments discussed. There is still much research to do on sleep and memory, but looking into ABNs effects with odor stimuli could provide interesting results to further our understanding, as well as identifying the specific tasks taking place in different stages of sleep.


References

Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2019). Cognitive neuroscience: the biology of the mind. W.W. Norton & Company.

Shanahan, Laura K., and Jay A. Gottfried. "Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory ..." Web. 6 May 2021.

University of California - Riverside. "How the brain consolidates memory during deep sleep: Using a computational model, study explains how hippocampus influences synaptic connections in cortex." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 14 April 2016. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/04/160414214830.htm>.


Applying a Feminist Lens to Nociception

In recent years, there has been a push for gender-inclusivity in all aspects of science, including clinical diagnosis and treatment. Unfortunately, decades of sexism in the treatment of women cannot be easily undone, but there is hope. Applying a feminist lens to neuroscience and nociception can assist in reducing sexism at the clinical level regarding ailments such as chronic pain, dysmenorrhea, and more.

A study from Hellman et al. (2020) investigated the connection between dysmenorrhea and chronic pelvic pain through quantitative sensory testing that measured each woman’s pain threshold. Researchers hypothesized that dysmenorrheal women with comorbid bladder pain sensitivity would have lower bodily pain pressure thresholds, as well as impaired conditioned pain modulation. Ultimately, results showed that provoked bladder pain hypersensitivity was strongly related to multiple pelvic pain symptoms, including poor conditioned pain modulation and increased pelvic and external body site mechanical sensitivity. Since dysmenorrhea often precedes bladder pain sensitivity, the authors conclude that further work may investigate the potential for women with dysmenorrhea to develop chronic pain since their target group met estimates for chronic pain conditions.

Although this study offers promising results and clarity on prior research in uterine pain and suggests that hormonal and surgical interventions may alleviate other visceral pain conditions, there is still another aspect of pain to consider: recognition and treatment of pain. A study from Zhang et al. (in press) investigated the actual estimation of others’ pain and how gender bias effects that. The authors hypothesized that at equivalent levels of pain facial expression and self-reported pain, male patients would be perceived to be in more pain than female patients, as well as more likely to be given pain medicine in higher dosages. Further, the authors predicted that female patients would be prescribed psychotherapy instead of medication, and that gender bias and stereotypes would influence pain treatment. 

Unfortunately, Zhang et al. (in press) found that women’s pain was underestimated compared to men’s, and that female patients would benefit more from psychotherapy. Further, a perceiver’s pain-related stereotypes influenced their pain estimation, even in controlled conditions for pain facial expression and self-reported pain. Although there is a limitation in the number of participants, there is potential for further studies to expand on the method of the present research to increase generalization to other populations. The authors conclude that sex bias has an influence on the recognition and treatment of pain, with women’s pain often underestimated and undertreated.

Looking at the results of these studies combined, it is evident that there is a lack of literature expanding on nociception regarding conditions associated with women, as well as a broader understanding of how the initial acknowledgement of pain influences subsequent treatment. Studies such as the one from Hellman et al. (2020) show promise in expanding research in this area, but Zhang et al.’s (in press) work reveals that there is still a long road ahead for true gender equality at a clinical level. Applying feminism to neuroscience--specifically in nociception--allows for increased awareness about gender bias and draws attention to the concerning lack of literature on neurological issues that center women’s pain. 

Sleep and Memory

 

    It is not uncommon to hear students talking about the "all-nighters" they have done to study for an exam. Although it may be tempting to do so, many studies have shown that memory consolidation heavily relies on sleep. So pulling that "all-nighter" may not be the best idea after all. Interestingly, many studies in the field of neuroscience have looked into this process of memory consolidation during sleep and how it could possibly be enhanced. Targeted Memory Reactivation, or TMR, has been an area of interest over recent years due to its potential to non-invasively enhance someone's memory while they're sleeping. 
    In an article by Laura K. Shanahan and her colleagues, they review multiple past studies that have broadened our knowledge on TMR. She describes TMR as the "practice of sensory cues to influence memory consolidation." The article reports a study done by Rasch and his colleagues, where they found that odors have can help with declarative memory consolidation during slow-wave sleep. A follow-up study showed that the hippocampus was receptive to olfactory stimuli during slow-wave sleep. Rasch’s study paved the way for many future studies, including one from Ritter and his colleagues, where they found that TMR had a positive effect on creativity levels.
In one of the most recent studies, Whitmore and colleagues tested the efficacy of TMR. However, rather than testing the effects of olfactory stimuli, researchers used auditory cues to test whether TMR during sleep could improve participants' face memory. They did so by testing participants in either a Japanese-history class or a Latin-American-history class. Here, participants learned the names of multiple people. Participants viewed 80 face-name pairs while they also listened to the spoken names. Before sleeping, their face recognition and name recall abilities were tested. After the participants were sleeping for around 7.3 minutes, TMR was initiated by playing the same background music from class along with half of the names previously learned by the participants. Results found a positive correlation between increased memory and duration of stage N3 sleep (slow-wave sleep). In other words, they did find they did find a significant improvement in memory because of TMR, but the extent of this improvement depends on how much uninterrupted N3 sleep a person gets. 
    Overall, memory is an important component of our everyday lives. Whether this involves studying for a test, remembering the names of people you just met, or remembering how to read the words in this article. The continued research behind TMR has shown many promising results that could possibly be integrated into our everyday lives. 

References 

Shanahan, L. K., & Gottfried, J. A. (2017). Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain. Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation, 335–346. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45066-7_20
 
Whitmore, N., Bassard, A. M., & Paller, K. A. (2021). Targeted Memory Reactivation of Face-Name Learning Depends on Ample and Undisturbed Slow-Wave Sleep. BioRxiv. Published. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.16.431530

Dysmenorrhea and the Body: More Research on Understudied Phenomenon

 

In their 2020 study “Dysmenorrhea Subtypes Exhibit Differential Quantitative Sensory Assessment Profiles” by Hellman et. al. researchers looked at the relationship between dysmenorrhea and provoked bladder pain, and sensory sensitivity. Dysmenorrhea is the medical term for menstruation pain. This study found widespread sensory sensitivity in women with dysmenorrhea and provoked bladder pain, and also called for more research into this extremely understudied area (Hellman 2020).

            Because this study was published so recently, there has not yet been time for the scientific community to respond these findings. However, there have been other studies that look more deeply at the mechanisms of dysmenorrhea and investigate why it caused the widespread sensory sensitivity found the Hellman study. One review paper by Barcikowska et. al. wanted to investigate cytokins and other proinflammatory factors and their role in primary dysmenorrhea (Barcikowska 2020). They found previous research that looked only at biochemical reactions between the endocrine, vascular, and immune systems lacking. Another study by Li et. al. looked at the association between dysmenorrhea and chronic pain, both pelvic pain (like the Hellman study) and nonpelvic pain. The team reported a positive association between the two factors (Li 2020).

            While the Hellman study was published too recently for there to be anything more recent in direct response to the questions it posed, there have been other studies and reviews that research the phenomenon of dysmenorrhea and its relationship other conditions of the body. Not only does this highlight the need for further research in this area, but it shows that future research needs to be done on the effects of dysmenorrhea as well as the cause of heightened severity in some women.

 

References:

Barcikowska, Z., Rajkowska-Labon, E., Grzybowska, M. E., Hansdorfer-Korzon, R., & Zorena, K. (2020). Inflammatory Markers in Dysmenorrhea and Therapeutic Options. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(4), 1191. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17041191

Hellman, K. M., Roth, G. E., Dillane, K. E., Garrison, E. F., Oladosu, F. A., Clauw, D. J., & Tu, F. F. (2020). Dysmenorrhea subtypes exhibit differential quantitative sensory assessment profiles. Pain, 161(6), 1227–1236. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001826

Li, R., Li, B., Kreher, D. A., Benjamin, A. R., Gubbels, A., & Smith, S. M. (2020). Association between dysmenorrhea and chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 223(3), 350–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2020.03.002

 

 

Aromatic Associations

Aromatic Associations

Laura Shanahan’s article “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain'' provides a unique perspective into the extensive impact the olfactory system has with memory. The use of sensory cues, known as targeted memory reactivation, or TMR, is utilized in this study in order to find effective translational research uses to solidify and manipulate declarative and emotional memories. In a similar lens, a recent article was published by the University of Fukui “Smell you later: Exposure to smells in early infancy can modulate adult behavior” explored the phenomenon in which newborn mice “imprint” onto scents that ultimately affect their future social behaviors. With the knowledge that mammals such as mice and humans are both social animals, the long term impact found in the olfactory association is a topic that is necessary to understand due to its impact on social behavior. 

Dr. Shanahan’s research not only correlates memory to the olfactory system, but implements the use of such methods into the sleep cycle to find potential effects that would result from such exposure. Using both positively and negatively perceived aromas, subjects were trained, exposed in their sleep, and recorded once again once awoken to find detectable differences that would be a result of the sleep exposure. An interesting aspect that I found in the study was the fact that the use of TMR led to promising results in fear extinction when previously exposed to negative olfactory stimuli.

Dr. Nobuko Inoue along with their team explored a question that also pertained to the use of smells in memory retention. Specifically, this correlation was discovered between newborn mice and the odors they are exposed to during a critical period. This critical period, only lasting a week, is a result of the interaction between Sema7A, a signaling molecule, and the receptor PlxnC1. During this time, scents that are detected are all positively correlated due to the release of oxytocin from this interaction. Although this appears to be a good system, difficulty arises when newborn mice are exposed to an aversive scent, the mice will now be conflicted for the rest of its life due to the positive correlation from imprinting and the negative correlation from their natural response. Although this exact pattern has not been seen in humans, it is important to note that this critical period may apply to other sensory inputs that can lead to long term effects to newborns. Ultimately, the importance of the olfactory system in animals such as mice and humans extends beyond its immediate use, and extends itself to the implementation of memories that may affect those exposed for their entire life.  Although much of the process in either studies are heavily unknown, the relation between potential fear extinction and its similarity to Dr. Nobuko Inoue’s research on the impact of negative olfactory stimuli during the critical period of newborn mice is interesting to see. 

Work Cited:

Shanahan, Laura K., and Jay A. Gottfried. “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain.” Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics, 2017, pp. 335–346., doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45066-7_20.

“Smell You Later: Exposure to Smells in Early Infancy Can Modulate Adult Behavior.” ScienceDaily, ScienceDaily, 13 Apr. 2021, www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/04/210413110645.htm.

ERP Effects in Infant Brain

Infants social bonding is strongly supported by mother’s face and voice recognition. Though, other aspects of the external world play an important role in development. Prior studies suggested that odor, and more especially maternal odor, is a significant influence: it can produce a soothing effect on crying, reduce anxiety during medical procedures and impact perceptual and cognitive processing. However, little is known about the precise role of maternal odor in early development.

In the recent study “Maternal odor reduces the neural response to fearful faces in human infants” (2020), researchers investigated how infant brain responses and emotional expression may be impacted when exposed to maternal odor. The research was conducted by using electroencephalogram (EEG) on seven-month-old infants and comparing exposure to happy and scary faces. The chosen age of the objects of study was seven-months of age since during that stage of life infants are able to distinct emotional facial expressions. To analyze the impact of maternal odor, researchers used two controls:

* Control 1: exposure to either no specific odor or an odor different from the infant’s mother
* Control 2: exposure to their mother’s odor

The results showed that infants exposed to Control 1 expressed a fear response and the infants exposed to Control 2 did not, which suggested that maternal odor has a significant impact in infants’ social perception.

What I found most interesting about this study, is that researchers quantified the response to fear signals by measuring the amplitude of the Nc, an event-related potential (ERP) component. The Nc component of ERP is frequently studied during infancy because it is related to the attention control (Conte et al., 2020) and the amplitude is believed to increase in response to fearful faces (Jessen, 2020).

Likewise, Conte et al. also investigated infants’ ERP responses (in this case to faces and objects) in developing infants for their research study denominated “Face-sensitive brain responses in the first year of life” where their focus of study was mainly the amplitude variation and cortical localization, and the results suggested that the Nc component differs for faces and objects and is dependent on the attentional state.

Both of the mentioned studies highlight the importance of attention, perception and recognition and how ERPs responses are related to them. Sensory processing is critical for pathological diagnosis but may be even more important to detect certain mechanisms during infancy to prevent or improve cognitive disorders. At that point of life, brain is still developing, and events are not fully encoded; thus, much of the brain development depends on the stimuli exposure during early childhood. Since recording and analyzing ERPs seems to reveal wide information about the brain activity in infants, it is crucial to continue investigating their action mechanisms to design treatments that enhance mental engagement, sense development and stimulation.
 
References
 
Conte, S., Richards, J. E., Guy, M. W., Xie, W., & Roberts, J. E. (2020). Face-sensitive brain responses in the first year of life. NeuroImage, 211, 116602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116602
Jessen S. (2020). Maternal odor reduces the neural response to fearful faces in human infants. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 45, 100858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100858

The Relationship Between Odor and Memory

 The Relationship Between Odor and Memory 

For many centuries, the connection between memory and odor has been studied through various means. A person’s sense of smell has been said to produce the most vivid forms of memory, more than visual imagery or sound. The sense of smell can have a powerful influence on the consolidation of associated memories and can clarify how sensory-rich memories are able to be stored in our brains. Studies have shown evidence for the sense of smell being highly connected to memory, in both slow-wave sleep and in Alzheimer’s Disease. 

In the study “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain,” Laura K. Shanahan and Jay A. Gottfried reviewed human literature on the ability of sleep-borne odors to selectively target memories, which influences both declarative and emotional memory consolidation. Shanahan and Gottfried (2017) observed a study where subjects participated in a visuospatial learning task that was paired with a rose odor. After this task, the subjects were put to sleep, and either the same rose odor or an odor vehicle was presented to them in alternation. After waking up, the researchers found that subjects performed better on the memory post-test when they were presented with the rose odor during slow-wave sleep. This finding determined that memory enhancement was specific to slow-wave sleep, and illustrated how odors amplify the consolidation of associated declarative memories during sleep. The researchers also concluded that the hippocampus was activated to a greater level when presented with the rose odor during slow-wave sleep, indicating that the hippocampus may be receptive to olfactory sensory processing. 

An article published on ScienceDaily called “Scientists uncover new connection between smell and memory,” from the University of Toronto, explains the process underlying the strong connection between memory and odor, and the loss of smell in Alzheimer’s Disease. Similar to the article from Shanahan and Gottfried (2017), the authors here say that there is a strong connection between a sense of smell and memory, and examine the hippocampus as a structure important to memory representation. In this study, researchers looked at the function of the anterior olfactory nucleus (AON) in mice. The mice with a disconnected pathway between the AON and hippocampus returned to previously smelled odors to sniff them for longer time periods. This result illustrates how the degeneration of the AON indicates an inability to detect previously observed smells. Early detection of Alzheimer’s may be more feasible now due to smell tests, although further research still needs to be conducted to perfect this. In relation to the article from Shanahan and Gottfried (2017), the emphasis on the connection between sensory olfaction and memory is highlighted, showing two different experiments where this relationship is present. Furthermore, both articles point out the fact that further research needs to be done on each respective topic regarding the relationship between olfaction and memory, suggesting that this topic is still fairly new and much more can be done to explore this connection. 

Although much more work needs to be completed to fully understand the mechanisms underlying olfactory sensation and memory, the research from Shanahan and Gottfried (2017) and the neurobiologists from the University of Toronto provide strong evidence that a person’s sense of smell is the most powerful form of memory consolidation. Even though certain findings, like the hippocampus in slow wave sleep, and the AON in Alzheimer’s patients are not fully developed yet, improvements and new technological methods can help scientists understand further processes regarding connections between olfaction and memory.


Sources: 


Gottfried, J.A., Shanahan, L.K. (2017). Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain. Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation, 335-346. https://doi.org./10.1007/978331945066720


University of Toronto (2018). Scientists uncover new connection between smell and memory. ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/07/180723155726.htm

Modification of Emotional Connections

    Post-traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and similar conditions such as depression and anxiety have a significant impact on people’s daily lives around the world.  PTSD, specifically, often is correlated with strong emotional connections to an event.  This connection can be extremely strong, and people often report total submersion into the memory of the event during an episode.  The root cause for the distress and cause of dysfunction in daily life is the emotional response when triggered back to the event.  The ability to remove these memories, or alter the emotional attachment to them, would be a significant step forward in the treatment of psychiatric disorders.
    Researchers are working on such an ability with the development of optogenetics.  This invasive medical procedure allows for extremely targeted modifications in synaptic strength.  Previously, researchers have been able to implant false memories into mice by tagging neurons of memory-engram regions in the mice’s hippocampus that were activated during a fear context.  They then later activated these neurons in a different context and elicited a fear response from the mice.  Perhaps more interestingly, researchers were also able to deactivate and then later reactivate memories.  This was done through optogenetic stimulation in the lateral amygdala.  The memory was deactivated via long-term depression which had disturbed the associative memory created earlier.  Researchers were then able to recover the memory via long-term potentiation the following day.  Impressively, researchers have been able to do repeat these results with well-established memories in mice by inhibiting CA1 hippocampal neurons when exposing the mice to a cue established over four weeks prior.  Similarly to the research done prior, these effects were temporary and reversible via stimulating the neurons in the CA1 hippocampus.  There are more possibilities with optogenetics than simply turning memories on and off, they can be manipulated as well.  We are capable of making valence modifications to memory, such as turning a sad one to a happy one.  This does not alter the memory itself but alters our emotion connected with it.  When we recall a memory engram, which is encoded in the hippocampus by the dentate gyrus, the valence of an elicited response could be altered through reassociation with a new unconditioned stimulus of an opposite valence.
    Emotional responses to stimuli studied by Dr. Foci, in his work “Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: Evidence from temporal-spatial PCA”, may have a unique connection to the future of optogenetic studies.  We consistently have emotional responses to the stimuli around us, and this may become highly taxing on those in high-stress work environments such as doctors and soldiers.  By targeting and flagging specific neurons, we may someday be able to turn on and off the emotional component of our connection to the world around us.  This may be highly beneficial for those who suffer from PTSD and similar anxiety disorders who may suddenly become overrun with emotion due to a triggering event.  Optogenetics may play a helpful role in psychiatric treatment through memory modification and through valence modifications in order to preserve memories but reverse negative emotional aspects of them.

https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11569-020-00377-1

Think with your nose!

 Think with your nose!


The term memory can be defined as something remembered from the past, a recollection of sorts. However, we have come to know that memory is more than just a collection of the past. Sure, that is a part of it, but numerous studies and experiments throughout the fields of neuroscience and psychology has taught us that there are many variants of memory. We can begin with three basic levels of memory: sensory memory, short-term (working) memory, and long-term memory. When we think about the word memory, most often people immediately refer to long-term memory, which was defined earlier as collection of the past. What do these collections entail? Our brain is an amazing work of biological machinery with the ability to sort these memories and code them into different groups, or types of memories. It is not often that we randomly think about our past memories, yet there is always something to trigger a set of memories which allows us to remember our past experiences. 


In the article “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain”, researchers Shanahan and Gottfried analyze how certain memory can be consolidated through the use of olfactory cues. Now most of us would think that we tend to recall something after seeing a picture we can recognize of hearing a familiar voice, but most of us wouldn’t think right away that memories can be triggered via olfactory stimulation. Their research focused on how smells could improve memory. The researchers asked participants to complete a set of tasks. The experiment group was given certain odors while completing the task and while they would sleep. The research showed that showed that when participants completed tasks with a certain odor and were then presented with the same odor during their sleep, they demonstrated better performances overall when compared to the control group which didn’t receive the olfactory stimulation during their sleep. These results indicate that olfactory stimulation can play a role in improving memory consolidation, a theory that has been previously studied and agreed upon. 


Not only do olfactory cues help with memory consolidation in humans, it can also help improve working memory in other species as well. A research study conducted by Ka Ho and Roberts showed that certain odors improved the episodic memory of dogs. Episodic memory is the long-term memory for remembering events and experiences. When the dogs were tested with odor stimuli, they were able to show an improvement in memory in terms of what, when, and where they remembered certain things. Although it was difficult to address whether or not they showed improvement in the what, where, and why separately, it showed there was improvement in their memory regardless of whether they were grouped together or not. 


It is fascinating to see how far research and experiment has taken us to see what the brain is capable of and how much the brain is capable of doing. We still have yet to see understand the entirety of the brain, but what we have learned so far is quite exemplary. There are so many neuronal connections that we wouldn’t expect to make ourselves. For example, in this case, the temporal lobe contains the olfactory cortex and the hippocampus (responsible for memory), both of which can be related as we have discussed in this article. Not only can we see these associations in humans, but many other animals as well. It is interesting to see how unique the human brain is, but also very much similar to the brains of other species. More importantly, it will be exciting to discover more about the brain in future studies.  


Works Cited

Lo, Ka Ho, and William A. Roberts. “Dogs (Canis Familiaris) Use Odor Cues to Show Episodic-like Memory for What, Where, and When.” Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol. 133, no. 4, Nov. 2019, pp. 428–441., doi:10.1037/com0000174. 

Shanahan, Laura K., and Jay A. Gottfried. “Scents and Reminiscence: Olfactory Influences on Memory Consolidation in the Sleeping Human Brain.” Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation, 2017, pp. 335–346., doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45066-7_20. 



SSRIs for Comorbidity of Chronic Pelvic Pain and GAD in Women?


According to Anxiety & Depression Association of America, Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) “is characterized by persistent and excessive worry about a number of different things” and is diagnose “when a person finds it difficult to control worry on more days than not for at least six months” and exhibits at least three associated symptoms, like having an increased heart rate or having trouble sleeping.  People who have GAD struggle to control their worry which may hinder other areas of their social, educational, or occupational lives.  Based on data from the National Institute of Health of the United States, an estimated 5.7% U.S. adults experience GAD at some point in their life.  In adults, women experience GAD at almost twice the rate that men do, at 3.4% and 1.9%, respectively.  

In “Dysmenorrhea subtypes exhibit differential quantitative sensory assessment profiles” Hellman et al. examined menstrual pain in reproductive aged women.  Although they were not directly studying anxiety, their data showed that “anxiety and somatic symptoms were significantly greater in all symptomatic groups”.  This means that the groups of women they studied who had another pelvic pain condition also showed a greater comorbidity rate of anxiety compared to healthy controls.  

Practical Pain Management, an all-in-one journalistic website about pain and pain management, published an article in 2021 titled “The Complex Intersection of Pelvic Pain and Mental Health in Women” written by Dr. Kathryn A. Witzeman.  There is limited research in this field (also acknowledged by Hellman et al. in their study) but Dr. Witzeman states that based on the existing research that there is a strong association between pelvic pain disorders and mood disorders, including anxiety.  She hypothesizes that “this increased comorbidity between persistent pelvic pain and mood disorders, as well as with other pain disorders of disparate body regions, may be influenced by disruptions in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and autonomic nervous system, which contribute to the regulation of stress and influence the perception of pain”.  Serotonin is a neurotransmitter that has many roles, including mood regulation and the perception of pain.  The HPA and serotonin directly impact each other, although how they do so has not been well-defined.  What if there was a treatment that could help both conditions?

For women suffering from pelvic pain and anxiety, looking at a type of antidepressant could be an option for managing symptoms of both issues.  Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRIs) are a type of antidepressant that increase the serotonin levels within the brain.  They are a much more safer option compared to addictive opioids and typically cause fewer side effects compared to other classes of antidepressants.  There is very little current research that exists on SSRIs and pain management.  This could be a new avenue for researchers to look into to help women suffering from pelvic pain and GAD.  



References:

Chronic Pain. Chronic Pain | Anxiety and Depression Association of America, ADAA. (n.d.). https://adaa.org/understanding-anxiety/related-illnesses/other-related-conditions/chronic-pain

Kathryn A. Witzeman, M. D. (n.d.). Pelvic Pain and Mental Health Disorders in Women: Which Comes First? Practical Pain Management. https://www.practicalpainmanagement.com/complex-intersection-pelvic-pain-mental-health-women

Hellman, K. M., Roth, G. E., Dillane, K. E., Garrison, E. F., Oladosu, F. A., Clauw, D. J., & Tu, F. F. (2020). Dysmenorrhea subtypes exhibit differential quantitative sensory assessment profiles. Pain, 161(6), 1227–1236. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001826 

Complex-intersection-pelvic-pain-mental-health-women