Although we
would like to believe that every child begins with an equal chance at pursuing
a healthy future filled with an abundance of knowledge, this may not be the
case as demonstrated by the research conducted by Dr. Daniel A. Hackman on the
relationship between socioeconomic position and the health of children. Dr.
Hackman felt the need to examine socioeconomic position due to its inevitable
nature to have an effect on many aspects of life such as access to social
resources or general life opportunities in the future. Additionally, the
complexity that socioeconomic position encompasses, where it isn’t just about
money, but resource availability, parental knowledge, neighborhood environment,
made the topic that much more interesting for Dr. Hackman to delve into. For
his studies, Dr. Hackman had primarily two areas of interest, executive function
and stress reactivity.
In his first
study, Dr. Hackman focused on the idea that parental care may play a role in
stress reactivity. Using the Trier social stress test, Dr. Hackman’s research
team measured the amount of cortisol in the participants’ saliva samples and
the results were most consistent with either his emergent/cumulative or
child-adolescent persistent models which both suggest a potential earlier
childhood origin and relevance in childhood. Through the results of his first
study, Dr. Hackman was able to conclude that lower parental reactivity
predicted a more “blunted” stress response in that children were more likely to
show a lack of physiological mobilization which may lead to risks in health
later on.
The second study
he worked on placed much emphasis on working memory because of its importance
in academic progress, health behaviors, and control over emotions. For this
second study, Dr. Hackman examined the relationship between parental education
and working memory and the results showed that differences in working memory
due to varying amounts of parental education could be seen both early on in
development and towards the end of adolescence. Now, while the differences were
pretty consistent, they were not massive. The association of working memory and
parental education exhibited the child-adolescent persistent model which
suggested an earlier childhood origin for the impacts perhaps maybe even before
the age of 10 which could have health implications later in life.
Lastly, in his third
study, Dr. Hackman looked at the association between working memory and income
and the results revealed that the differences in working memory maintained the
same rate of change, implying a closeness to the child-adolescent persistent
model, thus, an earlier childhood origin for the impact. With the information
from his studies, Dr. Hackman hopes to find more information about other
mediators of the differences seen as well as possible areas where intervention
or even prevention could be incorporated.
Now, in relation
to the impact socioeconomic status may have on childhood development, I came
across an interesting article highlighting the potential relationship between
the lengths of telomeres in children and the education level of mothers.
According to the article “Early Study Links Mothers’ Education level to
Telomere Length in Newborns”, author Nicholas Weiler discusses the findings of
a study conducted by Janet Wojcicki, PhD, MPH at UCSF. At present, medical
researchers are well-aware of the fact that telomeres can shorten due to stress
and therefore, may contribute to a “shorter lifespan” or even an “increased
risk for…diabetes, obesity, and cancer” (Weiler). However, the potential for
telomeres of children to shorten or lengthen based on their mothers’ education levels
is a bit more surprising. The mother’s education levels may have had an effect
due to the higher tendency to have more stress as a result of lower education
levels which are often connected with lower socioeconomic positions. Dr.
Wojcicki’s study was conducted with smaller sample group focused on Latina
women and their children. Their results revealed that increased maternal
education, meaning an education that was past the high school level, resulted
in a longer TL by around 500 base pairs. The reported p-value was less than
0.01, suggesting statistical significance (Wojcicki, Olveda, Heyman, Elwan,
Lin, Blackburn, and Epel). Thus, Dr. Wojcicki’s research was able to conclude
that increased maternal education may have a positive effect on TL length. The
results of the study have led researchers to consider “providing resources even
earlier” so as to compensate for the stresses mother with lower education may
face and the corresponding TL shortening that could occur (Weiler).
Moreover, the
results of Dr. Wojcicki’s study may be even more significant because earlier
this year, another research team at the University of Sydney was able to
publish their findings on the relationship between the length of telomeres in
early childhood and the thickness of arterial walls in early childhood. The
study was able to conclude that the shorter telomeres in younger children could
serve as predictions for one of the earlier signs of vascular disease, “increased
arterial thickness”, by as early as age eight (Weiler). Therefore, if future
studies could link the early information we have about the potential link between
socioeconomic status and early childhood telomere length, and the kinds of
health risks that can be predicted through examination of telomere lengths, the
medical field may be able to work towards creating more preventative measures
so as to limit these potentially detrimental conditions.
Link to blog:
Works Cited
Cartoon image of differences in
likelihood of catching a cold in children depending on socioeconomic position.
Digital image. MedicalXPress. Carnegie Mellon University, 1 Nov. 2013.
Web. 9 Dec. 2015. <http://medicalxpress.com/news/2013-11-children-socioeconomic-status-susceptible-colds.html>.
Hackman, Daniel A., and Martha J.
Farah. "Socioeconomic Status and the Developing Brain." Neuroscience
Seminar Box Site, 8 Jan. 2009. Web. 9 Dec. 2015. <https://luc.app.box.com/neuroscienceseminar/1/5608531521/45509088401/1>.
Hackman, Daniel A., Laura M.
Betancourt, Nancy L. Brodsky, Lara L. Kobrin, Hallam Hurt, and Martha J. Farah.
"Selective Impact of Early Parental Responsivity on Adolescent Stress
Reactivity." Neuroscience Seminar Box, 13 Mar. 2013. Web. 09 Dec. 2015.
<https://luc.app.box.com/neuroscienceseminar/1/5608531521/45509095665/1>.
Skilton, Michael R., Shirley
Nakhla, Julian G. Ayer, Jason A. Harmer, Brett G. Toelle, Stephen R. Leeder,
Graham Jones, Guy B. Marks, and David S. Celemajer. "Result Filters."
National Center for Biotechnology Information. U.S. National Library of
Medicine, 24 Sept. 2015. Web. 09 Dec. 2015.
<http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26405259>.
Weiler, Nicholas. "Early
Study Links Mothers' Education Level to Telomere Length in Newborns." UC
San Francisco. UCSF News Center, 4 Dec. 2015. Web. 09 Dec. 2015.
<https://www.ucsf.edu/news/2015/12/345286/early-study-links-mothers-education-level-to-telomere>.
Wojcicki, J. M., R. Olveda, M. B.
Heyman, D. Elwan, J. Lin, E. Blackburn, and E. Epel. "Cord Blood Telomere
Length in Latino Infants: Relation with Maternal Education and Infant
Sex." Nature.com. Nature Publishing Group, 3 Dec. 2015. Web. 9 Dec.
2015. <http://www.nature.com/jp/journal/vaop/ncurrent/full/jp2015178a.html>.
Programming Language Assignment Help UK
ReplyDeleteBest Writing Help In UK
Best Essay Writing Services Help UK
Assignment Help
Essay Help On Grid Point Weather
Assignment Writing Services On FILE4NET
Assignment Writing Services On Sologet
Assignment Writing Services On rainbowtv
Essay Writing Services On scioly
Essay Writing Services On houseboatmagazine