Serotonin is a powerful neuromodulator that contributes to
feelings of elevated mood, well-being, and overall happiness. Low serotonin
levels and activity reduction is often an indicator of psychiatric disorders
such as depression, anxiety, bipolar depression, and other mental diagnoses. Though antidepressants such as
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) drugs are considered a valuable
form of treatment in the psychiatric world, the positive impacts of drugs that
are classified as SSRIs and the way they are marketed in the pharmaceutical
world are widely exaggerated and problematic. Though SSRI drugs such as
Citalopram, Sertraline, and Fluoxetine have been successful for many patients
suffering from depression, there is a lack of scientific understanding on why
these drugs work. Neuromodulators are defined as brain chemicals that modify
neuronal dynamics, excitability, and synaptic function.
According to
Crockett's article, SSRIs are known to enhance serotonin function by blocking
its reuptake and prolonging its effects on postsynaptic receptors.
Psychiatrists have grown comfortable and accustomed to the idea that SSRIs are
undoubtedly the best form of treatment by restoring serotonin levels to normal
and being a remedy for chemical imbalances. In Crockett's article, the effects
of an SSRI known as Citalopram are studied on deontological moral judgments in
multiple domains including harm and
care, empathy, punishment, and fairness and reciprocity. Mary J. Crockett and her team of researchers began their study on
participant's judgment on the moral permissibility of harmful actions, which is
observed in three different scenarios. On November 5th, Dr. Joe Vukov from
Loyola University Chicago gave a presentation elaborating on Molly Crockett’s
scientific study, Neurochemical
Modulation of Moral Judgment and Behavior. Dr. Vukov expresses how moral
sentiments and judgments are influenced by neuromodulators, specifically
serotonin’s effects when it is either enhanced or depleted. Serotonin was the
chosen neuromodulator in this study due to its flexibility in control of
behavior (Crockett, p. 237). Yet, Crockett’s study ultimately proves that
neuromodulator levels are extremely difficult to measure in humans, as there is
no scientific consensus on what constitutes healthy levels of serotonin in an
individual. Healthy levels of serotonin cannot simply be defined by SSRIs since
abnormal neural chemistry may persist in individuals. Environmental and other
outlying factors may be a causation for this.
David Healy, a professor of psychiatry at Bangor University
in Wales and author of Let Them Eat
Prozac, further supports this notion and emphasizes that the “link between
serotonin and depression is a ‘myth’ that continues to be perpetrated by the
pharmaceutical industry" (Sifferlin, 2015). In addition, Dr. Victor I.
Reus, a professor in the department of psychiatry at the University of
California, San Francisco, illustrates that though there is no doubt that SSRIs
work: “we don’t have a comprehensive and holistic understanding of why they
work” (Sifferlin, 2015). Though serotonin is recognized for monitoring various
social behaviors, regulators used to normalize serotonin levels, SSRIs, are not
well understood and impact individual moral behavior and judgment in complex
ways. While Crockett’s article proposes the idea that there is a lack of
scientific evidence on the neuromodulator serotonin and its influence on
individual judgment, the article “Is the Link Between Depression and Serotonin
a Myth?” also highlights the idea that SSRIs may have potential drawbacks when
used as a treatment for mood disorders.
Serotonin’s influence on deontological moral judgments in
the domain of harm and care was
studied and participants' judgment on the moral permissibility of harmful
actions was observed in three different scenarios. In the first case, the drug
citalopram, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, was administered on
participants. Next, participants received a noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor
drug called Atomoxetine. In the third scenario, participants received a placebo
pill. The effects of Citalopram, Atomoxetine, and placebo on judgments were
compared in three different scenarios: neutral scenarios that contained no
moral content, “personal” moral scenarios in which harmful actions were
emotionally salient, and “impersonal” moral scenarios in which harmful actions
were not emotionally salient. While neutral and impersonal scenarios observed
no differences across treatments on judgments, personal scenarios seemed to increase
harm aversion. This was because participants were more deontological,
indicating that they were less likely to induce harm on one individual to save
many other individuals. Next, researchers also studied how drug effects
interacted with individual differences by splitting participants into low and
high empathy groups. The researchers discovered that there is no effect of
citalopram on judgments in the low-empathy group, yet there is a great effect
in the high empathy group.
Next, researchers studied how serotonin affects morality in
the aspect of fairness and
reciprocity through data collection using the Ultimatum game, studying how
costly punishment can be affected by manipulation of the serotonin system.
Researchers studied the effects of enhancing serotonin function with citalopram
on costly punishment in the Ultimatum Game. While comparing the effects of
Citalopram and Atomoxetine, researchers discovered that citalopram, a selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitor, influenced decision-making while Atomoxetine, a
noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor, did not affect decision-making. The effects
of Citalopram were strongest in participants with high empathy; citalopram
reduced the rejection of unfair offers producing the opposite effect to acute
tryptophan depletion. In addition, brain regions associated with motives for
revenge were studied in depth through neuroimaging study. Scientists observed
that there is evidence that actively delivering punishment has motivational
value. When people were punished for unfair behavior, there was an increase in
activity in the dorsal striatum. These scientific findings suggest that if
depleting serotonin increases punishment behaviors by enhancing the
motivational value of punishment, then we should observe that serotonin
depletion increased activity in the striatum in individuals who punish others. Consequently, scientists observed that
serotonin depletion increased activity in the dorsal striatum during
punishment, and individual differences in the neural effects of depletion were
correlated with individual differences in the behavioral effects of depletion.
Evidence from Sifferlin’s article suggests that each
individual's neural chemistry is more complex, and providers must place
individual attention on each patient to understand which method of treatment is
most effective for mood disorders such as depression. Alternatives to SSRIs may
be used, such as older tricyclic antidepressants which may have fewer effects
of impairment in one’s daily life. Simultaneously, Dr. Vukov’s presentation on
Crockett's article emphasizes that neuromodulation on moral judgment and
behavior is contingent on factors such as stress, each individual has complex
neural chemistry influencing their decision making and moral judgment, and
individuals don’t recognize when their actions may be morally impermissible or
harmful.
Dr. Norman Sussman, a professor in the department of
psychiatry at New York University, states "Some of the older drugs may
work better with fewer quality- of-life-impairing effects.” Ultimately, I
believe that psychiatrists and scientists must look at alternative options
rather than constantly relying on SSRIs for treatment. Some psychiatrists don't
even allow patients to elaborate on feelings and problems they are experiencing
in their personal lives. Instead, they make a hasty judgment to prescribe their
patients with antidepressants/ SSRIs such as Prozac, Zoloft, etc. It is
important to take a more holistic approach to avoid potentially harmful side
effects that SSRIs may have. Every individual has different neural chemistry,
and the solution is not as simple as SSRIs restoring abnormal serotonin levels.
SSRIs are also often overprescribed. Healy describes how there is no doubt that
serotonin plays a role in the treatment of depression and that it is "not
irrelevant"; yet the problem persists with the idea that "the market
boom of SSRIs raises questions about why physicians would put aside clinical
trial evidence in place of ‘plausible but mythical’ accounts of biology.”
Evidence from Crockett's study indicates that moral
judgments are not fixed, but are contingent on neuromodulator levels and
stress. Neuromodulator levels are constantly changing, so it is difficult to
see precisely what the influences of this flux are on our moral judgments and
behavior in real-time. This suggests we cannot be fully aware of what is
guiding some of our morality, and that it is sometimes being guided by
neuromodulators that do not seem relevant to morality. Determining which state
is “neutral” for establishing a neutral ethical baseline is a short value
judgment that would be influenced by the same neurochemical factors. Crockett’s
research suggests that our moral judgments, including those about what might be
normal functioning or healthy moral behavior, are largely influenced by our
neurochemicals. If this is the case, then perhaps we should not be as confident
in our normative evaluations, which in turn seems to suggest we should not be
as confident in our evaluations of what counts as “correct” morality of normal
behavior. Sifferlin’s article leads us to wonder about the justification of the
idea of norms that underlie the decision to treat people in the first place. In
the coming years, it will be crucial to systematically investigate these questions
and debate their significance for morality. The idea of a neutral physiological
state can only arise with the insertion of moral judgment because there is no
scientific basis for it.
Works Cited
Crockett,
Molly J. “Morphing Morals.” Moral Brains, 2016, pp. 237–245.,
doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199357666.003.0011.
Sifferlin,
Alexandra. “The Link Between Serotonin and Depression Is a Myth, Psychiatrist
Says.” Time, Time, 21 Apr. 2015,
time.com/3829565/is-the-link-between-depression-and-serotonin-a-myth/.
No comments:
Post a Comment