In the paper "The human visual system's assumption that light comes from above is weak", researchers Yaniv Morgenstern et al. studied the ability of the human visual system to perceive 3D shape using the light-from-above prior assumption and lighting cues. Surprisingly, their results indicated that the preconceived notion of the light-from-above prior playing a critical role in perception was weak. On the contrary, Morgenstern et al. found that the human visual system has a much heavier reliance on lighting cues when determining shape. The influence of lighting cues was so profound, that researchers discovered the weakest lighting cues had a similar effect as the light-from-above prior on perception. They were able to reach that conclusion following a series of shape-from-shading conditions, while simultaneously measuring the angles of effective lighting direction relative to the prior. Under the weak lighting cue condition, researchers observed a 66º circular standard deviation from the width of the light-from-above prior. A comparison was then drawn between this condition and the circular SD of lighting directions that are observed in our everyday lives, which was stated as 54º. The comparatively small variation in the angles of effective lighting direction in relation to the prior under weak cues and those apart of everyday life were therefore, key in establishing the strong influence lighting cues have on perception.
Similar to the work by Morgenstern et al., an article reviewed by Emily Henderson titled "Humans may perceive thickness based on image characteristics" for News Medical Life Sciences detailed the manner in which researchers Ohara, Koida, and Kim went about tackling the topic of perception. Interestingly, they approached from a completely different perspective and illustrate yet another manner in which the visual system works to perceive characteristics of 3D shape. In their research, they studied the effects of image cues (such as transparent properties) on 3D shape perception--specifically, a judgment of how thick an object appears to be under different conditions. Employing a computer driven experimental method, Ohara et al. discovered that participants displayed a higher tendency to judge transparent objects as flatter, in comparison to those with a glossy appearance. Furthermore, following an analysis of these images, researchers were then able to determine that the most precise estimations of thickness were linked to regional variation. In short, similar to how Morgenstern et al. observed angles of lighting direction relative to the prior for weak cue conditions, Ohara et al. found that regional variation from local contrast played a key role in participants' judgments. Although it was mentioned in the article that a clear cause for misinterpretation is not yet known, it is interesting to consider the fact that the thickness of transparent objects may have been more likely to be misjudged due to the lower local contrast caused by this transparency. This is supported by the lack of pixel contrast in a confined part of an image that displays a transparent object. The lack of pixel contrast makes it harder for the participant to discriminate shape and therefore, misinterpret thickness.
Regardless, both articles successfully illustrated different manners in which the human visual system is able to perceive 3D shape. As mentioned in the Henderson article, further understanding of how misinterpretations in shape may arise is crucial in developing technology that will aid those who are visually impaired. For example, Henderson mentions the use of a computational model in the form of glasses, or even mobility devices. However, the different approaches to perception in both articles provide two avenues for aid. Not only would technology-driven glasses serve the purpose of helping those who may not be able to discriminate between bumps and flat areas of a surface, but the incorporation of a tint to the glasses may help those who suffer from not being able to determine the shape of an object as a result of insensitivity to lighting cues. Our ability to provide variations of aid therefore, reinforces the intricacy of the visual system when it comes to 3D shape perception.
References
Morgenstern, Y., Murray, R.F., Harris, L.R. (2011). The human visual system's assumption that light comes from above is weak. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(30), 12551-12553; DOI:10.1073/pnas.1100794108
Henderson, E. "Humans may perceive thickness based on image characteristics". News Medical Life Sciences, 11 February 2021, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20210211/Humans-may-perceive-thickness-based-on-image-characteristics.aspx
My husband was diagnosed with early onset Parkinson's disease at 57.his symptoms were shuffling of feet,slurred speech, low volume speech, degradation of hand writing, horrible driving skills, right arm held at 45 degree angle, things were tough for me, but now he finally free from the disease with the help of total cure ultimate health home, he now walks properly and all symptoms has reversed, he had trouble with balance especially at night, getting into the shower and exiting it is difficult,getting into bed is also another thing he finds impossible.we had to find a better solution for his condition which has really helped him a lot,the biggest helped we had was ultimatehealthhome they walked us through the proper steps,am highly recommended this ultimatehealthhome@gmail.com to anyone who needs help.
ReplyDelete