Wednesday, December 14, 2016

Memory Modification is Coming Sooner Than You Think

Memory Modification






            With science fiction on the rise there are many movies or TV shows that are testing the waters of what the world would be like in a few year. One show like this is Black Mirror, a Netflix original. Black Mirror tests the waters on what would society be like if we had a certain machine to lets say download our memories or to record everything we see. In one of the episodes we see an implant in our brains that allows us to record all our memories and allows us to play them back whenever. Through out the episode we see how this machine can hinder or increase our social interactions, and how their “new” society views memories.
            Our technologies nowadays are nowhere near that level of memory collection, but science has been able to innervate certain aspects of our memories. Joe Vukov who is a Philosophy professor at Loyola University of Chicago came to talk about the ethics of new studies being done that modify human’s memory. There are drugs now that allow for victims with PTSD to not remember their experiences that well. Theses treatments are consider MMS which rely on the protein kinases MS to dull the amygdala whenever the topic of the PTSD is being thought of.

            Joe Vukov came in to talk about the main ethics of the memory modification using drugs. There are two main sides on this argument; we have the alarmist and the modifiers. The alarmists believe that memory modification is unnatural because the memories we have form who we are, and even bad memories are needed for the exact reason that they are bad. The alarmist bring up a few good points on how we would not be the people we are today without the memories of our past. The modifiers believe that memory modification is morally permissible as long as no harm is done to anyone. They also have a few good points especially relating to the well being of humans, those with PTSD. They also believe that our identify is malleable and memory modification will just be another aspect of our identities. Mr. Vurkov agreed on some aspects of both groups so he came up with Treatmentalism. Treatmentalism is on the basis of allowing to use memory modification as a treatment until it restores someone to a normal level of functioning. Meaning memory modification would be able to be used on patients with PTSD, and treat their memories to stop hindering them in daily life. But on the other hand memory modification should not be use to erase a bad memory such as getting a D on a test. Treatmentalism is still in the works of becoming a full fledge ethical group but the main aspect is to get human to a normal psychological level of functioning.

Neuroethics: Biomedical, Legal, and Social Implications of Neuroscience


In recent years, the field of neurology has grown rapidly. But, with neurological advancements, more ethical issues arise as we increase our ability to image and modify neuronal activities.
Neuroimaging involves the use of imaging technologies to create scans of living brain tissues. But it is not limited to biomedical uses. It can also be displayed as a form of art. Neuroimaging is often used to visualize morphological characteristics hidden underneath the skin. Artist Marta de Menezes utilized fMRI imaging to visualize regions of the brain that are highlighted when a specific task is performed [1]. Scans obtained from fMRI were used to create functional portraits of many individuals and displayed online for public viewing. However, artists need to be careful when displaying this private information online because recently researchers have been studying the biological foundation of personality using fMRI imaging. There is experimental evidence that neuroimaging data, combined with life history and genetic information, can precisely predict behaviors in many individuals [2]. Therefore, it can be extremely dangerous to publicly expose images of neuronal activity in the form of an art. In the future, behavioral trait predictors may negate an individual’s notion of free will in our society.
Neuroimaging also has the potential to be used as a lie detector. Its results are interpreted on the idea that lying requires more cognitive effort and more oxygenated blood [3]. However, it is important to remember that images do not exist in a direct relationship with a behavior. Neuroimaging may help us understand what makes a behavior more tempting, but it does not illustrate criminal intent in individuals. Also memory modifications may make it even trickier to use neuroimaging as a lie detector in criminal court cases. Memory modification technologies (MMTs) have evolved greatly in recent years. MMTs are used to modify neuronal circuits that govern specific memories [4]. These technologies provide many new treatment options for individuals suffering from trauma or phobia. For example, MMTs can be used to erase a traumatic event from an individual’s memory to help them live a healthier life. Or it can be used to alter an individual’s memory to ease their fears or phobias. MMTs can be helpful in many cases but its use has to be regulated in order to avoid any unforeseen negative consequences. It is critical for MMTs to specifically target only certain memories and neurons as to not alter or damage other important memories. Also, guidelines for memory modifications need to be implemented to regulate individuals who can utilize this technology. If an individual commits a criminal act and uses a MMT to erase this memory, criminal court systems would have a hard time determining whether or not this individual was guilty of committing the crime.
Neurological technologies have a myriad of practical applications in real life, but they are governed by their own set of ethical issues. Neurotechnologies have a huge potential for misuse and abuse. For this reason, individuals need to educate themselves to better understand safety and ethical issues concerning MMTs and neuroimaging. With regulations and guidelines, high ethical standards will likely be upheld in the field of neuroscience.

References

1.     De Menezes, Marta. "Functional Portraits." Marta De Menezes. Moshi Moshi, 2012. Web. 14 Nov. 2016.
2.     Canli T. When genes and brains unite: ethical implications of genoming neuroimaging. Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy (J Illes, ed.). New York: Oxford University Press; 2006, p. 175.
3.     Illes, J. A fish story? Brain maps, lie detection, and personhood. Cerebrum. 2004 Fall; 6(4):73-80.

4.     Liao, S. Matthew, and Anders Sandberg. "The Normativity of Memory Modification." Neuroethics 1.2 (2008): 85-99. Web.

Forget Me Not: Ethical Considerations of Memory Modification






Imagine a future where you can modify your memories, what would be the first memory you would get rid of? Would it be the cringe-worthy time when no one showed up to your sweet sixteen party? The first time you got into a car accident? Or maybe the time someone close to you passed away? The possibilities are endless! But at what point do we draw the line? Would we elect to forget our first heartbreak? Or the first time we failed an exam? Should we even consider going down this route? After all, our memories essentially define our existence and shape who we are.

Dr. Vukov brought up many of these points when he gave a lecture to our neuroscience seminar class on the ethics of memory modification. He opened up the lecture with the idea of Brave New World Neuroethics— an approach to understudying ethical concerns of neuroscience research and innovation in a way that is “brand new”. Basically, Brave New World Neuroethics claims that we cannot utilize past generations’ morality to understand this seemingly new moral territory. It makes sense, doesn’t it? Morality seems to be an ever-changing entity that molds itself to fit the mores and folkways of the current generation. However, Dr. Vukov disagrees with this. Yes, neuroscience will bring up ethical questions in new ways, but at the end of the day the questions brought up can be simplified down to questions that have always plagued philosophers—Is it the right thing to do? What is right? What is a good life? How does it look like? What is our true self? Is it constant or dynamic?  What is an identity? The questions can go on, but I think you can see that the crux of the ethical concerns remained the same throughout time. 

Memory modification may seem like it is lightyears away from our grasp, but current research shows potential for expediting it. In his lecture, Dr. Vukov discusses several propranolol studies; propranolol is a beta-blocker used to control heart rhythm, treat angina, and reduce high blood pressure. Interestingly, beta-blockers also seem to function as memory consideration blockers for emotionally charged memories. The first study he discussed was the Initial Study (Cahill, et al., 1994). Participants in the study were either given a propranolol or a placebo; they were then shown slides that were paired with either an emotional or neutral narrative. The participants were tested a week later to see if they could remember the slides. The researchers discovered that memory for the neutral slides did not differ, however, memory for the emotional slides was significantly heightened for the placebo participants in comparison to the propranolol participants. What this seemed to suggest is that propranolol dulled the consolidation for the emotionally charged memories.

The Fear Conditioning Studies ( Kindt et all., 2009) fear conditioned the control and propranolol groups through the use of terrifying images and a neutral tone. After some time, they administered propranolol to the test group and exposed both groups to the conditioned fear memory. The researchers waited until the propranolol in the propranolol group was flushed out of their systems to expose both groups to the tone once more and then recorded their fear responses. The outcome lead to a stunning conclusion, propranolol diminished the fear response in a way that was significantly different than the control group.  Researchers concluded that in addition to dulling consideration of emotionally charged memories, propranolol blocked the reconsolidation of the memories. In layman terms, this means that propranolol can change memories that were already formed.


The Narrative Recall Studies (Brunet et al., 2008) used participants with PTSD to see how propranolol would affect their traumatic experiences. They were asked to write up a script of their traumatic experience and then listen to the script; they were then either given propranolol or a placebo. They were invited back a week later to listen to the script once more. As you may have guessed, the propranolol group showed a diminished fear response that—like previous study—suggested that beta-blockers seem to block emotional memory reconsolidation.

In addition to propranolol and other beta-blockers, there is another drug that seems to affect memory consolation and reconsolidation: CI-994. CI-994 is a relatively new drug that was created as an inhibitor for HDAC2—“ a master regulator of the expression of neuroplasticity  genes.” (Tsai, 2014). In her article, “Drug Tweaks Epigenome to Erase Fear Memories”, Virginia Hughes writes about Li-Heui Tsai, the neuroscientist who is leading work behind the CI-994 drug. In one study, Tsai demonstrated that administering CI-994 resulted in an unlearning of fear memory in mice 30 days after the fear memory was formed. According to Tsai, it only took one dose and there were zero side effects—as in, other memories were still intact. The fact that the feared memory was unlearned after such a substantial time is incredible because PTSD, especially the severe forms of it, is a lifelong battle. Traditional therapies—such as extinction therapy—only work in recently acquired memories, making it somewhat useless for a large demographic of PTSD sufferers. CI-994, along with the beta-blockers already on the market,has the potential of changing the lives of many people by moving research one step closer to a memory modification super drug.

While is all seems amazing and revolutionary, it is important to step back and consider the ramifications. People who are known as “the Alarmists” would argue that we should never use drugs, or any other method, to alter our memories permanently because they are a core part of who we are. Our true selves cannot be true if our memories are incomplete. Additionally, there is value in bad memories. For example, what if the world forgot what happened during the Nazi regime? Or if we forgot the struggles that plagued the African American community during segregation? We better ourselves and our communities by building upon painful memories to ensure that they never occur again.

On the other hand, people like “the Modifiers” would claim that, as long as it does not harm anyone, it is morally acceptable to alter memories. They claim that the good of the person comes before anything else. For example, victims of violence and abuse may want to forget the traumas they experienced, or soldiers with PTSD may want to forget what they have seen and done. It seems a bit cruel to make them endure such painful memories if there is a way to get rid of them, or even just dull them. Additionally, the true self is constantly evolving. If outside forces such as trauma can modify our true self, then how can we claim that we should not have a drug to act as an outside force modifier? Are our true selves not true if we craft them through choice?

I personally believe in the idea of “treatmentalism”. Essentially, I believe that people should only use memory modification for circumstances that invoke a tremendous amount of trauma that interfere with normal, every day functioning. I would like to emphasize that last point because trauma is subjective, someone going through a divorce may feel as if the world is falling apart. However, with normal therapy and some time, there is a great likelihood of that person coming to terms with the divorce and moving on. Should the person not get over it and resort to destructive behaviors—such as self harm or alcoholism—then I would recommend therapy for those behaviors first, such as inpatient therapy, and then recommend the modifier drug as a last resort. 


Alarmist, Modifier, Treatmentalist—what do you label yourself as? Do the benefits outweigh the costs?


References
Hughes, (2014). Drug Tweaks Epigenome to Erase Fear Memories. Retrieved December 14, 2016, from http://phenomena.nationalgeographic.com/2014/01/22/drug-tweaks-epigenome-to-erase-fear-memories/

Vukov, J. Enduring Questions and the Ethics of Memory Modification. Powerpoint slides. Retrieved from Presentation_November_29.PDF

Images:
Http://psychology.iresearchnet.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/Memory.jpg [Digital image]. (n.d.).

Http://integratedlistening.com/wp-content/uploads//2016/01/460669515.jpg [Digital image]. (n.d.).


Memory Modification: Too Good To Be True?

















In today’s society, we try our best to make sure we prevent as many embarrassing moments as we can. Falling in front of a crowd or being caught singing in front of the mirror might make many of us cringe. Many of us try to stifle our bad memories and refrain from remembering them. Once they resurface, we are forced to relive that moment in our life again. From common embarrassing mistakes to life altering tragedies, bad memories preoccupy the minds of us all.
S. Matthew Liao’s article titled “The Normativity of Memory Modification” tackles a new ethical concern in the world of neuroscience. The article states that researchers are studying the relationship between Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Memories that stem from strong emotional background tend to be remembered more strongly. For instance, remembering a family member’s death is easier than remembering what one ate for dinner last week. A catastrophic memory can easily resurface and this can cause extreme anxiety. Researchers are looking to apply memory modification to soldiers in war. Veterans are susceptible to extreme cases of PTSD due to all of the inhumane and heart wrenching events that they witness each day. Liao talks about the malleable nature of memories. Memories are often manipulated and altered depending on when we recall them. He states that each time we remember something, our recall of the event is a little bit different from the last.  Although there is a significant amount of research that is taking place, scientists are not at all close in modifying human memories at will (Liao, 89).
An issue that people may have in developing Memory Modification Technologies (MMT) is isolating memories. Memories are interconnected and many parts of the brain come together to encode memories. When manipulating one specific memory, other memories will in turn also be affected. Another problem that the author mentions is the idea that memory modification can leave to a world of mistrust. Memories function as evidence of events that occur in our lives. If we were to alter them in any way, the memories would lose their true meaning. Our perception of events would not be accurate and we would be living a lie. This proves to be an ethical concern as altering memories can lead to a multitude of problems. If two people were recalling a certain event, then they might burst out into an argument as each side believes their story is correct. This can lead to a multitude of problems that would affect all aspects of our lives including the judicial system. By modifying or blocking memories, we would be altering our true self. The experiences we go through, the hardships we face, the obstacles that force us to be stronger, are the ones that shape us to be who we are. If we were to take away the memories that crushed our hearts, it might change who we are. Without the pain that we once went through something so intense, our minds might force us to not care about certain issues.
Similarly, in an article titled “Does Memory Modification Threaten Our Authenticity”, the author Alexandre Erier talks just about that. He mentions that MMT raises a number of questions on the authenticity of the human self. He goes as far as to say that with this technology, might give society a false sense of too much control of our lives. Erier outlines four scenarios which show why MMT might be an unethical idea. In all four scenarios, we see that each person forgets who they actually are and take on a false life. Even though some of these choices may lead to a more positive outlook on life, once they alter their memories, they lose who they truly are.

When looking at both of these articles, it is clear to see that although Memory Modification Technology seems like a fascinating step forward in the world of neuroscience, it presents a multitude of ethical concerns. From what I have learned about MMTs, I have concluded that I do not agree with the use of MMTs. I believe that our memories shape us to be who we are. Without our bad memories, we may have a more positive outlook on life, but we may also change our true self. Often times, our parents remind us to never forget our roots and to carry on traditions. The same principle applies for MMTs as memories allow us to hold on to who we are.

Works Cited:
Erler, Alexandre. "Does Memory Modification Threaten Our Authenticity?" Neuroethics. Springer Netherlands, Nov. 2011. Web. 15 Dec. 2016.

Liao, Matthew, and Anders
Sandberg. "The Normativity of Memory Modification." Springer Science, 10 Apr. 2008. Web. 11 Dec. 2016.