As
a child, I wasn’t always best at school. In fact, I dreaded sitting down to
study for tests and had excessive trouble with memorization. My mother used to
tell me that if I slept with my notes under my pillow, I would wake up knowing
all the information. Regrettably, learning new information is not that simple. While
putting notes under your pillow is not going to magically make you smarter when
you wake up, it is true that memory is consolidated during sleep. This is why academic
institutions discourage students from pulling “all-nighters” or sleeping for
periods less than 6 hours. Sleep is a necessary component to enhancing memory storage.
While memory
consolidation already occurs during sleep, how can we amplify
these already existing mechanisms?
Dr. Iliana Vargas’
research focuses on strengthening individual memories by reactivating
them during sleep. During her research, it was found that reminders of smells
or sounds during sleep can be used to “target the reactivation and
strengthening of individual memories.” Targeted Memory Reactivation (TMR) utilizes
sounds and odors to strengthen the already present memory consolidation that occurs
during slow wave sleep. The underlying mechanism for the strengthening of
memories is referred to as “neural replay”: newly learned information corresponds
to a specific pattern of neural firing that then continuously recurs throughout
sleep, ultimately strengthening the neural pathways and memories. Aware that
memories gain stability during sleep consolidation, Vargas investigated whether
“reinstating a learning context (an odor) during slow-wave sleep enhances
retrieval of spatial information learned in that context.” The study required
subjects to learn 50 images and associate those images with a specific sound
and location on a map displayed on the computer screen. For example, an image
of a cat would be presented with a meow, while a kettle would be presented with
the sound of a whistle. Researchers would then reintroduce the sounds during the
non-REM portion of sleep as the participants napped. The results of the study
showed that memory processing can be highly specific and that information presented
during sleep influences ensuing retrieval of information when awake.
Similarly, Dr.
Bjorn Rasch, a biopsychologist at the University of Fribourg in Switzerland,
utilized TMR with the memory game Concentration. Subjects were taught to
associate images of cards with specific locations presented on a computer screen.
Then throughout the study period, researchers released the smell of roses into
the environment, essentially linking the odor to the material being learned.
Rasch found that those who were exposed to the smell of roses during sleep
displayed greater accuracy during the recall period where participants had to
remember the placement of the cards.
The use of TMR is
important because it may be able to help us alter how we think, advance our
abilities when learning new information, and perhaps even lessen trauma
associated with past experiences. Focusing on sleep and memory consolidation, Dr.
Penny Lewis, a neuroscientist and sleep scientist at Cardiff University in
Wales, describes memories as multisensory experiences. The idea that memories
are multisensory experiences is clearly seen in both Vargas’ and Rasch’s
research, where senses are presented as being powerfully tied to our memories
and even propel retrieval of them. While TMR is only in its infancy, it’s
potential impact is boundless. Scientists are currently trying to find a
relationship between TMR and the process of consolidating emotional memories. Dr.
Lewis is attempting to see whether triggering traumatizing events from our past
during sleep can make these events less distressing. Participants in Lewis’
study are presented disturbing pictures (i.e. car crashes) with sound cues. Participants
find these images less troubling when the memory of the image is reactivated
during sleep through TMR. Lewis attributes this phenomenon to the lack of the neurotransmitter
noradrenaline during rem sleep. Noradrenaline is responsible for our bodies
physiological fear responses, such that of increased heart rate, sweating, and
pupil dilation. Lewis believes that replaying the memory of the disturbing
images in the absence of noradrenaline, allows the participant to experience
the memory without the averse emotional response, and consequently, be able to
dissociate the memory from the emotion. While it is too early to draw
conclusions regarding the future of TMR, it’s not hard to imagine the use of
TMR as a treatment for trauma disorders such as PTSD.
Works
Cited
Winkle's,
Van. "Targeted Memory Reactivation Will Make You Smarter as You
Sleep." Inverse. Inverse, 09
Feb. 2017. Web. 03 May 2017.
Image retrieved from: https://static01.nyt.com/images/2017/04/09/fashion/09COVER1/09COVER1-superJumbo.jpg
No comments:
Post a Comment