Monday, May 1, 2017

The Problem with All Nighters



Time and time again, many friends and I have been warned of the real costs of pulling all nighters. But between the hectic schedule of every undergraduate, lack of sleep, and the mighty need to procrastinate, there never seem to be enough hours in the day, or night, to finish studying for that neuroscience final. Contrary to popular student belief however, the subsequent nightly rite of passage that ends up with one in the library past all hours, could be hurting one’s grade more than helping it and more importantly, hurting one’s mental health. In fact, numerous studies have documented that sleep actively supports the formation of long-term memory representation (Feld & Born, 2017) and that sleep deprivation harms memory (Havekes et al., 2016).
In Dr. Iliana Vargas’s recent seminar at Loyola University Chicago, she detailed an experiment which tested the strength of individual memories and memory storage after targeted memory reactivation during sleep. Participants were taught to associate unique object images with locations on a computerized grid and characteristic sounds before taking a nap. After waking, participants experienced these paired auditory-visual stimuli once more and asked to match each image with its associated location. It was found that object placements of objects cued by their sounds during sleep were more accurate than object placements of objects that were not auditory cued during sleep. This finding suggested that memory storage was enhanced via targeted memory reactivation during sleep (Rudoy et al., 2009).
The particular stage of sleep in which formation of normal spatial memory takes place in mice models was studied by Sylvain Williams, a researcher and professor of psychiatry at McGill University. Optogenetics, which allows for targeted control of a discrete population of neurons through light, was used to regulate hippocampal activity (hippocampus: critical in memory formation; ‘GPS’ system of brain). Mice were trained to spot novel objects in a given area, then put to sleep such that when mice were in REM sleep, memory-associated neurons were turned off by light pulses. Mice were unsuccessful in the previously learned spatial memory task the following day; compared to the control group, their memory seem impaired/ nonexistent. It was therefore determined, that the REM sleep (stage in which dreams occur) was critical for normal spatial memory formation in mice (States News Service, 2017)

Further research must be done in this area to determine the purpose of REM sleep in humans and its potential role in memory formation. However, if results obtained in Williams’ studies can be extrapolated to human, a little dreaming might do some students some good the night before a final!




Image: https://i.ytimg.com/vi/J62ZaFNZVu0/maxresdefault.jpg

Feld, Gordon B., and Jan Born. "Sculpting Memory during Sleep: Concurrent Consolidation and
Forgetting." Current Opinion in Neurobiology 44 (2017): 20-27.
Havekes, Robbert, Alan J. Park, Jennifer C. Tudor, Vincent G. Luczak, Rolf T. Hansen, Sarah L.
Ferri, Vibeke M. Bruinenberg, Shane G. Poplawski, Jonathan P. Day, Sara J. Aton, Kasia RadwaÅ„ska, Peter Meerlo, Miles D. Houslay, George S. Baillie, and Ted Abel. "Sleep Deprivation Causes Memory Deficits by Negatively Impacting Neuronal Connectivity in Hippocampal Area CA1." ELife 5 (2016): n. pag.
"Rapid Eye Movement Sleep: Keystone of Memory Formation." States News Service. N.p., 13 May
Rudoy, John D., Joel L. Voss, Carmen E. Westerberg, and Ken A. Paller. "Strengthening
Individual Memories by Reactivating Them During Sleep." Science 326.5956 (2009): 1079. 

No comments:

Post a Comment