Smelling
bitterness, hearing coldness, feeling color, seeing pitch, and tasting light. All
of these sensations fall under an umbrella of synesthesia. However, one may have a certain sensation that could trigger a
memory or response from a different sense. In a study conducted by researchers
at MIT, the worm Caenorhabditis elegans was discovered to have the ability to detect light through
its ability to taste hydrogen peroxide. Tasting hydrogen peroxide alerts the
worm to other reactive oxygen compounds and allows it to have a better sense of
awareness in its environment. All modes of light detection were formerly known
to involve a chromophore, however, this worm may lead to the discovery of light
sensitivity in a whole new realm. In this case, this adaptive light sensitivity
through taste serves as a defense mechanism towards harmful substances in the
environment. Researchers found that the worms stop eating when they are exposed
to hydrogen peroxide and when they are exposed to light, suggesting that both
light and hydrogen peroxide induce the same behavior. In the worm’s nervous
system, certain receptors (such as LITE-1) within a neuron are responsive to
the level of hydrogen peroxide as well as have a level of light sensitivity. This suggested that certain receptors detect
light indirectly by first “detecting reactive oxygen compounds generated by
light” (2). While this group of receptors is specific to invertebrates, humans
have an enzyme called peroxiredoxin that has the potential to play a role in
oxygen detection by light in the human eye.
Much like how the MIT
study describes an association with gustatory and light perception, a recent
study by Alex C. Keene and Pave Masek of the Mac Planck Institute of
Neurobiology delves deeper into the relationship between optics and gustatory
behavior. In particular, their study highlighted how optics or optogenetics
have the ability to activate or suppress taste behavior through memory. By exposing
Drosophila to various laser beams at different
regions on their bonding and by observing this influence on certain
subpopulations of taste neurons which then induced a certain behavior, researchers
were able to better understand the neural circuitry that defines behavior. The researchers
found that after using IR-lasers to activate bitter-sensing neurons, the flies
had a similar suppression response when exposed to natural quinine. It’s as if they had
been conditioned towards remembering a bitter response through the use of
lasers. This discovery
is comparable to the first study in that while both light and hydrogen peroxide
cause C. elegans to suppress food
consumption, both laser activated bitter sensations and naturally bitter
substances induce Drosophila to
suppress their consumption as well. Both of these studies draw a connection
between what was once separated as optical perception and gustatory sensation,
tiptoeing on the line of what could trigger synesthesia and suggesting a possibility
that synesthesia could be conditioned into organisms over time.
Keene,
A. C., Masek, P., (2012). Optogenetic induction of aversive taste memory.
Neuroscience, 222, pages 173-180.
Trafton, A. (2015, January 29). Tasting light.
Retrieved May 1, 2015, from
http://newsoffice.mit.edu/2015/hydrogen-peroxide-taste-receptors-sense-light-0129
No comments:
Post a Comment