Wednesday, October 10, 2012

Not born to rock, but determined to.


For most of us, music is an ever present element in our lives. Look down any busy city street and you witness a spectrum of musical activity, from the haggard guitarist panhandling on the corner, to the high-end department stores blaring upbeat pop in front of their stores to attract customers, and not to mention the countless number of pedestrians toting oversized (and overpriced) headphones, lost in their own personal soundtrack as they go about their day. Whether we take an active interest in it through singing or playing an instrument, or if we are simply passive consumers, music permeates our daily experience. Differences between the passive and active participants are glaringly obvious to anyone. Most of us consider ourselves as either being musical or not. I know I’ve never considered myself at all musical. In my case, a childhood fascination with music was curtailed by a less-than-supportive orchestra teacher that informed me that I just “wasn’t cut out for it”. But for a select portion of the population, music has been an integral part of their lives since childhood. So the question that comes to mind is, what fundamental differences arise between those of us with the ability to produce music and those of us who cannot? A number of psychologists and neuroscientists have looked at this question and offered some fascinating insights into the contrasts between the “musical brain” and the rest of us. 

    One psychologist who has extensively studied the development of so called “musicality” is Gary Marcus, a cognitive psychologist at New York University. In his book, “Guitar Zero”, Marcus chronicles his adventure towards becoming a musician at the tender age of, well, thirty-eight. Marcus approaches a myriad of questions and topics on the subject of musical talent and learning with the rigor of a seasoned research psychologist, but is also able to bring the insightful, and often comical, musings of a man conquering his limitations. Marcus is, of course, not the only researcher interested in music and its relation to the brain. A recent article on the New York Times website summarizes the work of several researchers in the realm of auditory research. A majority of the article illustrates several examples of musical trainings benefit on the brain and how it may help facilitate various auditory tasks in subjects. Researchers at Northwestern have found that the effects of musical training are robust, in that their benefits persist even after training has stopped. Specifically, individuals who had received musical training at some point in their lives were more adept at recognizing things like pitch. The ability to recognize and discern differences in pitch is an ability that Marcus details throughout his book. As someone who studies evolutionary psychology as well, Marcus is particularly interested in the idea of someone possessing an “absolute pitch” or perfect pitch as it is known more colloquially, an ability that would seem to be aided by a certain confluence of genes that may lead to better auditory hardware. Evidence for this can be seen in studies performed on infants, in which the infants show an ability to recognize changes in pitch and rhythm. But a point made by both Marcus and the article is that this seemingly inborn propensity for music is nothing without training. Researchers at the University of California in San Francisco are attempting to tease out the contributions that genes make in the ability to recognize pitch, but have made clear that such abilities will only manifest themselves in the presence of musical training.


What is it about musical training that allows an individual to discern things like pitch? Well, like any type of learning, music training relies on the concept of brain plasticity, or the brain’s ability to rewire itself. The brain has an unparalleled ability to reshape itself as we learn and this reshaping leads to improved skills in areas such as those that are responsible for our ability to discern sounds. Differences in the brains of musicians vs non-musicians is a well documented phenomenon. Notable characteristics of the trained musician’s brain can be found in regions of the brain employed in music. Areas such as the planum temporale, an area that has been equated with pitch perception, have been shown to develop differently in musicians. These studies also can show us how areas of the brain with large roles in our other faculties can play a contributing role in music. For example the cerebellum, an area of the brain usually associated with motor coordination, is also responsible for the sense of rhythm necessary to produce music. Other brain areas can be dually utilized in music and our other facilities, which seems to be the case when studying the subject of language.

Both language and music rely heavily on rhythm and pitch in their comprehension and so are likely relying on the same processing areas in the brain. Talk to any native English speaker trying to learn Chinese and they will tell you how important pitch and rhythm can be to a language. Tonal languages, like Chinese, rely on different intonations and pitch when trying to confer meaning. Marcus’s contribution to this area of discussion draws heavily on his evolutionary background. Marcus points to the medley of brain areas involved in music that have been observed over the years with the help of brain-imaging technologies. What he and other neuroscientists can infer from that is that there is no single module in the brain responsible for music. Instead, it draws on areas of the brain that have evolved to be implemented in other areas of perception and have just recently become utilized in music. Researchers at Northwestern’s Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory are addressing questions similar to the ones that Marcus addresses in his book. They are looking at tasks that are likely implemented in both music and language learning, things such as working memory and making “sound-to-meaning connections”.

So what is the overall message we can draw from work by Marcus and others? I would like to think it is all about the idea of potential. We, as a species, can take parts of ourselves, of our brains, that evolved to do things completely outside the realm of music, and create works of art. Songs that persist in the collective milieu of culture for centuries. Songs that can inspire. Songs that can humanize. Songs that push the boundaries. I believe that, in most cases, the esteem that most societies have placed on musicians can be well founded. Musicians can take the very essence of human experience and put it to a melody, give it a rhythm, and make it accessible to anyone willing to listen. But there is no reason we cannot all do that in some way. It may not be in the way Jimi Hendrix or the Beatles have, but it can still be something profoundly real to you. Something that captures the very core of you, if only for a few brief minutes. I believe music, art in general really, can be our most veracious endeavor. And for people like Marcus, an endeavor that knows no “critical period”, no defined time to blossom. Marcus’s success in learning the guitar at such an age, however modest he made it seem, speaks volumes of potential and of the benefit of concerted effort. Such an effort can overcome the greatest barriers erected in our psyches by ourselves, or in some cases by others, and help us achieve the goals we once thought impossible, just as impossible as Marcus once believed it would be for him to rock.   


No comments:

Post a Comment