What is
the difference between a call and a song? Not calling your friend on the phone versus
sitting down with a guitar and playing “Dust in the Wind”, but rather, the
difference between the barking of a dog and the singing of a person. In his
article “Singing Mice May Join Human sand Songbirds As Vocal Learners,” Jason
Goldman states that a call is an innate sound that an animal makes and a song
is a collection of sounds that change in terms of acoustic qualities and
syntax. Humans are capable of producing both calls and
songs, but what about other animals? It has been believed for some time that many
birds and some mammals are capable of producing songs, and now there may be one
more little guy to add to the list.
Researchers in 2005 argued that mice are capable of producing not
only calls, but songs as well. This seems a little odd: I have never heard a mouse
produce more than a squeak, which seems to lack acoustic quality and syntax
variation. But when researchers looked beyond the spectrum of what the human
ear can hear, they found that mice produce ultrasonic sounds. After analyzing
the variation between these sounds, it appears that mice actually have some
acoustic quality and syntax variation.
But what does this mean for the evolution of sounds, language, and
music? Mice could be like rock stars, singing to attract the opposite
sex. They are by no means the prettiest animals on the planet, so maybe
a mouse that can woo his or her mate with a song is more likely to have a life
partner. It works for Gene Simmons, right? Gary
Marcus toys with this idea in his book Guitar Zero. In this book, Marcus comes to the conclusion
that the ability to produce music (a much more complex ability than the production
of a song by a mouse) is not a sure fire way to attract a mate. But
maybe a mouse’s ability to produce a song reflects the health of that mouse. Goldman cites
Holy and Guo’s 2005 study that investigated the effect deafness has on a mouse’s
ability to produce learned songs. They found that if a mouse becomes deaf their
songs diminish in quality. This phenomenon is also prevalent in humans; someone
who becomes deaf will not sound the same as they did before. As
Goldman proposes, this is due to the fact that in order to maintain song
quality one must be able to process auditory feedback. It is
possible then that when mice are looking for a mate, they try to find mouse
that can sing a sweet song to prove that they are healthy.
The fact that mice need auditory feedback in order to maintain
their songs parallels the fact that humans need auditory feedback in order to
make captivating music. As seen in Holy and Guo’s graph, three week
after mice are experimentally made deaf the pitch and frequency of their vocalizations are affected
(Goldman). In Guitar Zero, Marcus strongly advocates the idea that people can be fantastic
musicians without being able to read or write sheet music, as their ability to
listen to music they make is what allows them to make captivating sounds that
are both novel and familiar. A person can make music without being able to
read music, but they will have a tough time making music without being able to
hear the sounds that they produce. Hearing is vital for mice and humans
to produce learned vocalizations.
In
no way is a mouse’s ability to sing at all close to a human’s ability to make
music, but it is interesting to see the parallels between the two. Mice and humans both rely on the
ability to hear in order to maintain the quality of learned vocalizations. More importantly, we can now do
research with mice to possibly learn more about the evolution of music in
humans.
Goldman, Jason. "Singing Mice May Join Humans and Songbirds As Vocal Learners." Scientific America, 10 Oct. 2012. Web. 10 Oct. 2012. <http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/thoughtful-animal/2012/10/10/singing-mice-might-join-humans-and-songbirds-as-vocal-learners>.
No comments:
Post a Comment