The Glass Ceiling:
Holding Women Back Since 1984
The
term “glass ceiling” has been around since Gay Bryant put a name to it in 1984,
but since then we may have forgotten that it’s just a word. Before that time and ever since that
time women strive to overcome this “barrier.” But what is this barrier really other than a word? Does this
gap exist because men are more capable of moving up then women, or is it
something society has pushed on each gender? More specifically, women have a hard time excelling in STEM
jobs – science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. I recently came across two explanations
that support my ambiguous inquiry: 1) Society is shaping women to think that
they can’t pursue STEM careers, despite encouraging scores on standardized
tests, and, 2) Women have more career options than men, and therefore choose
jobs that truly interest them instead of just getting what they can.
A
New York Times article by Natalie Angier (2013) reviews a presentation that she
believes encompass this gender gap problem in STEM careers. Doctoral student Anthony Derriso
presented information concerning a study that he has been working on since
2009. The data comes from
self-report surveys given to ninth graders concerning a range of topics that
aim to reveal gender disparities.
An example question is: “are you likely to pursue a scientific career?” Sixty-one percent of the students who
responded “yes” (11%) were male even though self-report surveys showed no
differences in aptitude across gender (how they view their own math and verbal
skills; how engaged and supported they feel by teachers, family, and
peers). So the question we are
left with at the end of this article is “why the humungous difference in intent…when
given the choice, why do so many girls walk away from science and math?”
In
her book Pink Brain, Blue Brain, Lise Eliot explores similar issues of
gender inequality, taking the reader on a trip from the womb to
adolescence. Particularly relevant
to the article by Angier is a chapter that looks at the development of math and
science skills in young children, and how boys and girls progress
differently. She speculates an
answer to the questions posed by Angier and Derriso. Eliot proposes that these gender gaps in math and science
are “both innate and cultural…Without question, parents and educators can do
much more to raise girls’ interest and achievement in these important fields”
(207). Similar to Derriso’s
self-report task among ninth graders, Eliot notes that girls and boy do in fact
begin their academic career with comparable math and science skills, and also
that boys usually end up on top.
Here, it is suggested that the academic gender gap beings when girls
enter college. Eliot has found
that girls’ and boys’ math and science scores are comparable up until college
(yes, the boys are a bit ahead in test scores by now, but nothing
significant). Here, is where we need
to implement the change in these young girls. Referencing author Steve Olsen: “The paucity of girls at the
highest levels of competition is much worse in the United States than in other
countries…cultural factors keep girls from math” (214). Eliot believes that girls are just as
mentally apt in STEM fields/careers as boys, but that they are not socially
accepted.
In
March of 2013, Ming-Te Wang, Jacquelynne S. Eccles, and Sarah Kenny published a
study on individual and gender differences in STEM careers. They obtained findings along the same
line as the data Derriso had for his presentation, but their deduction rivals
Eliot's. The present
longitudinal study looked at 1,490 individuals when they were in twelfth grade
and again at thirty-three years old.
They wanted to know if students with high verbal and mathematical
abilities would be more or less likely to engage in a STEM career when compared
to someone who had high mathematics skills, but only moderate verbal
skills. It turns out that the
former group was less likely to obtain a STEM career, while the latter excelled
in STEM involvement. More
interesting yet, a majority of the participants in the high-high group were
female. The researchers’
conclusion took a different direction than previous studies: “The pattern of
gender differences in math and verbal ability may result in females having a
wider choice of careers, in both science, technology, engineering, and
mathematics (STEM) and non-STEM fields, compared with males.” Wang et al. also note that individuals
are compelled to gravitate to what they are good at, so it makes sense that
women are in non-STEM jobs because of how many options they have that are more
desirable.
There
is a lot of debate about the gender gaps that occur – nature or nurture? Is it a matter of aptitude or
intention? Can we overcome the
stigma of STEM careers or are women disinterested in these fields? There is a lot of conflicting evidence
out there, with more data being gathered every day.
_________________________________________________________________________
Angier, Natalie.
“Mystery of the Missing Women in Science.” The New York Times. 2 September, 2013. Web. 24 February
2014.
Eliot, Lise. Pink
Brain, Blue Brain: How Small Differences Grow into Troublesome Gaps - and What
We Can Do About It. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing
Company, 2009. Print.
Wang, M., Eccles
J.S., & Kenny, S. (2013). Not Ability but More Choice: Individual and
Gender Differences in Choice of Careers in Science, Technology, Engineering,
and Mathematics. Psychological Science,
24(5). doi: 10.1177/0956797612458937
No comments:
Post a Comment